SwePub
Sök i SwePub databas

  Utökad sökning

Träfflista för sökning "AMNE:(SAMHÄLLSVETENSKAP Psykologi) ;pers:(Sikström Sverker)"

Sökning: AMNE:(SAMHÄLLSVETENSKAP Psykologi) > Sikström Sverker

  • Resultat 1-10 av 178
Sortera/gruppera träfflistan
   
NumreringReferensOmslagsbildHitta
1.
  • Arvidsson, David, et al. (författare)
  • Changes in self and object representations following psychotherapy measured by a theory-free, computational, semantic space method
  • 2011
  • Ingår i: Psychotherapy Research. - : Informa UK Limited. - 1050-3307 .- 1468-4381. ; 21:4, s. 430-446
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • We propose a theory-neutral, computational and data-driven method for assessing changes in semantic content of object representations following long-term psychodynamic psychotherapy. Young adults in psychotherapy are compared with an age-matched, non-clinical sample at three time points. Verbatim transcripts of descriptions of the self and parents were quantified in a semantic space constructed by Latent Semantic Analysis. In the psychotherapy group, all representations changed from baseline to follow-up, whereas no comparable changes could be observed in the comparison group. The semantic space method supports the hypothesis that long-term psychodynamic psychotherapy contributes to sustained change of affective-cognitive schemas of self and others.
  •  
2.
  • Bäck, Emma, et al. (författare)
  • From I to We : Group Formation and Linguistic Adaption in an Online Xenophobic Forum
  • 2018
  • Ingår i: The Journal of Social and Political Psychology. - : Leibniz Institute for Psychology (ZPID). - 2195-3325. ; 6:1, s. 76-91
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Much of identity formation processes nowadays takes place online, indicating that intergroup differentiation may be found in online communities. This paper focuses on identity formation processes in an open online xenophobic, anti-immigrant, discussion forum. Open discussion forums provide an excellent opportunity to investigate open interactions that may reveal how identity is formed and how individual users are influenced by other users. Using computational text analysis and Linguistic Inquiry Word Count (LIWC), our results show that new users change from an individual identification to a group identification over time as indicated by a decrease in the use of “I” and increase in the use of “we”. The analyses also show increased use of “they” indicating intergroup differentiation. Moreover, the linguistic style of new users became more similar to that of the overall forum over time. Further, the emotional content decreased over time. The results indicate that new users on a forum create a collective identity with the other users and adapt to them linguistically.
  •  
3.
  • Hedberg, Per-Henrik, et al. (författare)
  • Investigating correct and incorrect statements of eyewitness testimony using semantic spaces
  • 2009
  • Ingår i: 21st Annual Convention of the Association for Psychological Science, San Francisco, 2009. ; , s. 81-81
  • Konferensbidrag (övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt)abstract
    • Studies have shown that descriptions of real and suggested or fabricated eyewitness memory may differ in ways that could be explained by differences in the formation and cognitive representations of these memories. However, the characteristics of an eyewitness’ description are likely to depend not only on the cognitive features of memory, but also on the witness’ social motivation to display meta-cognitive states to the investigator. For example, when a response to a question is delayed, this delay is open to several interpretations. It may mean that the respondent a) have difficulty understanding the question b) is retrieving the information c) is formulating her response d) is withdrawing from the interaction. To prevent misunderstanding, respondents need to account for their delays, uncertainties, and failures in answering. Linguistic research shows that when responding to general knowledge questions, people do this by using several markers (Brennan & Williams, 1995; Krahmer & Swerts, 2005; Smith & Clark, 1993). Respondents often use fillers like “uh” and “um” to indicate some trouble with processing. The fillers both signal the delay and offer a brief account for it. Lack of confidence can either be implied by rising intonation, or by hedges as “I think”, “I guess”, etc. Respondents can also account for delays with self-talk and explicit commentary. Both fillers and self-talk are showing the questioner that despite the delay, one is still actively trying to retrieve an answer. Both are ways to let the questioner in on how the retrieval is progressing, and show that the respondent is not uncooperative, ignorant, poor in judgment etc. Listeners have been shown to use these cues to make adequate assessments of the certainty or uncertainty of the speaker (Brennan & Williams, 1995; Krahmer & Swerts, 2005; 2006). This constitutes the Interactive view on question answering (Smith & Clark, 1993). The current research investigated whether similar communicative cues discriminated eyewitnesses’ accurate and inaccurate responses to questions about a crime event. Furthermore, we examined the extent to which groups differing in experience with judging eyewitness memory use these cues to estimate witness accuracy. Witnesses were videotaped while being interviewed about their memory of a simulated crime scenario. Responses to cued recall questions that provided correct or incorrect information about a specific detail were protocoled and scored with respect to linguistic and paralinguistic uncertainty cues (delays, fillers non-words/words, hedges, number of words). Results confirmed a higher frequency of “uncertainty” cues in witnesses’ incorrect as compared to correct responses. This finding points to the importance of taking the interactive component of the question-answering situation into account when analysing eyewitness memory. The eyewitness statements were presented to police detectives, chief judges, and lay-persons in written or in videotaped format. While all groups had difficulty determining accuracy, experienced police detectives, but not judges, had a better discrimination accuracy than lay-persons. Both professionals and lay-persons showed better discrimination when presented with statements in written than in videotaped format. This suggests that the visual format conveys information that interferes with the detection of valid accuracy cues.
  •  
4.
  • Petersén Karlsson, Kristina, 1968-, et al. (författare)
  • Gender differences in autobiographical memory : females latently express communality more than do males
  • 2019
  • Ingår i: Journal of Cognitive Psychology. - : Informa UK Limited. - 2044-5911 .- 2044-592X. ; 31:7, s. 651-664
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Gender differences have been found in several aspects of autobiographical memory (i.e. personally experienced events). For example, previous studies have shown that females’ autobiographical memories contain more communal and emotional expressions than do males. However, an important question concerns whether these differences can be observed both in the manifest content (i.e. what is actually said) and in the latent content (i.e. the underlying meaning of what is said). In the present exploratory study, we extended the current knowledge concerning gender differences in autobiographical memory by investigating the manifestly expressed words, as well as the latently expressed words in autobiographical memory descriptions. We observed an overall gender difference in the latent content of the autobiographical memories. Furthermore, females latently described their memories in more communal terms than males did. No other gender differences were found. Our results indicate that females’ autobiographical memories are more communally oriented than male's.
  •  
5.
  • Pietraszkiewicz, Agnieszka, et al. (författare)
  • The big two dictionaries : Capturing agency and communion in natural language
  • 2019
  • Ingår i: European Journal of Social Psychology. - : Wiley. - 0046-2772 .- 1099-0992. ; 49:5, s. 871-887
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Four studies developed and validated two dictionaries to capture agentic and communal expressions in natural language. Their development followed the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) approach (Study 1) and we tested their validity with frequency‐based analyses and semantic similarity measures. The newly developed Agency and Communion dictionaries were aligned with LIWC categories related to agency and communion (Study 2), and corresponded with subjective ratings (Study 3), confirming their convergent validity. Very low or absent correspondence between proposed dictionaries and unrelated LIWC categories demonstrated their discriminant validity (Study 2). Finally, we applied both dictionaries to language used in advertisements. In correspondence to gender stereotypes, male‐dominated jobs were advertised with more agentic than communal words, and female‐dominated jobs with more communal than agentic words (Study 4). Both dictionaries represent reliable tools for quantifying agentic and communal content in natural language, and will improve and facilitate future research on agency and communion.
  •  
6.
  • Söderlund, Göran, 1955- (författare)
  • Noise improves cognitive performance in children with dysfunctional neurotransmission
  • 2007
  • Doktorsavhandling (övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt)abstract
    • Research on children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) has shown that they are extremely sensitive to distraction from external stimuli that lead to poor cognitive performance. This thesis shows that cognitive performance can be improved if this external stimulus is smooth and continuous (e.g. auditory white noise). Control children attenuate their performance under such conditions. The first Study proposes the moderate brain arousal model (MBA). This neurocomputational model predicts selective improvement from noise in ADHD children. Noise through a phenomenon called stochastic resonance (SR), can be beneficial in dopamine deprived neural systems. The statistical phenomenon of SR explains how the signal-to-noise ratio can be improved by noise in neural systems where the passing a threshold is required. The second Study provides experimental support for the MBA-model by showing that ADHD children improve performance in a free recall task while exposed to auditive noise. Control children declines in the same condition. The third Study generalizes this finding among low achieving children, which it is argued have low dopamine levels. Noise exposure improves performance in low achievers, but inhibits performance in high achievers. The conclusion is that external auditory noise can restore low dopamine levels and thus improve cognitive performance. It is also proposed that dopamine levels modulate the SR effect; this means that low dopamine persons require more noise to obtain an SR effect. Both excessive and insufficient dopamine is detrimental for cognitive performance. The MBA model can be used to explain several shortcomings where changes in the dopamine system have been identified. The MBA model can also help create appropriate and adaptive environments, especially in schools, for persons with a deficient dopamine function, such as ADHD children.
  •  
7.
  • Seddigh, Aram, et al. (författare)
  • The association between office design and performance on demanding cognitive tasks
  • 2015
  • Ingår i: Journal of Environmental Psychology. - : Elsevier BV. - 0272-4944 .- 1522-9610. ; 42, s. 172-181
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • The physical office environment has been shown to be associated with indicators of both health and performance. This study focuses on how memory performance is affected in normal working conditions compared to a quiet baseline (with low amount irrelevant stimuli) in different office types, including cell offices, small open-plan offices, medium-sized open-plan offices and large open-plan offices. The results showed that the drop in performance from the quiet baseline to normal working conditions was higher in larger, compared to smaller, open-plan offices. However, contrary to our hypothesis we found that cell offices might have negative effects on performance comparable to those of large open-plan offices. These results indicate that employees in small open-plan offices, in comparison to large, have better possibilities to conduct cognitively demanding tasks and that cell offices might not be as advantageous as previously thought.
  •  
8.
  • Garcia, Danilo, 1973, et al. (författare)
  • Self-descriptions on LinkedIn: Recruitment or Friendship Identity?
  • 2018
  • Ingår i: PsyCh Journal. - : Wiley. - 2046-0252 .- 2046-0260. ; 7:3, s. 152-153
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • We used quantitative semantics to find clusters of words in LinkedIn users' self-descriptions to an employer or a friend. Some of these clusters discriminated between worker and friend conditions (e.g., flexible vs caring) and between LinkedIn users with high and low education (e.g., analytical vs. messy).
  •  
9.
  • Stille, Lotta, et al. (författare)
  • Language and gender : Computerized text analyses predict gender ratios from organizational descriptions
  • 2023
  • Ingår i: Frontiers in Psychology. - : Frontiers Media S.A.. - 1664-1078. ; 13
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Previous research has shown that language in job adverts implicitly communicates gender stereotypes, which, in turn, influence employees’ perceived fit with the job. In this way, language both reflects and maintains a gender segregated job market. The aim of this study was to test whether, and how, language in organizational descriptions reflects gender segregation in the organizations by the use of computational text analyses. We analyzed large Swedish companies’ organizational descriptions from LinkedIn (N = 409), testing whether the language in the organizational descriptions is associated with the organizations’ employee gender ratio, and how organizational descriptions for organizations with a majority of women and men employees differ. The statistical analyses showed that language in the organizational descriptions predicted the employee gender ratio in organizations well. Word clouds depicting words that differentiate between organizations with a majority of women and men employees showed that the language of organizations with a higher percentage of women employees was characterized by a local focus and emphasis on within-organizations relations, whereas the language of organizations with a higher percentage of men employees was characterized by an international focus and emphasis on sales and customer relations. These results imply that the language in organizational descriptions reflects gender segregation and stereotypes that women are associated with local and men with global workplaces. As language communicates subtle signals in regards to what potential candidate is most sought after in recruitment situations, differences in organizational descriptions can hinder underrepresented gender groups to apply to these jobs. As a consequence, such practices may contribute to gender segregation on the job market.
  •  
10.
  • Petersén Karlsson, Kristina, 1968- (författare)
  • Autobiographical Memory : Depending on sensory retrieval cue and gender
  • 2019
  • Doktorsavhandling (övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt)abstract
    • In my thesis I raised two questions: Does autobiographical memory differ i) depending on the sensory retrieval cue (Study I and II)? and ii) depending on gender (Study III)?Concerning retrieval cues, three unimodal cues (i.e., photographs, naturalistic sounds and odors) and one multimodal cue (i.e., the three unimodal cues presented simultaneously) were used to evoke autobiographical memories. The results demonstrated that the memories differed depending on retrieval cue. In particular, the olfactory-cued memories differed in semantic content and were from an earlier age in contrast to the other modalities. The visually and auditory-evoked memories differed less than expected in their semantic content and age distribution in relation to the multimodal condition. The multimodally cued memories could be described as being a combination of the three unimodalities, as illustrated by the semantic content and age distribution, though they were more similar to the visually and auditory-cued memories than to the olfactory-cued memories. One possible explanation for these results could be that we attend more to visual and auditory perceptions than to olfactory.With regard to gender, previous research has found gender differences in the manifest content (i.e., the actual words used) of autobiographical memories. However, to my knowledge none has investigated gender differences in the latent content (i.e., the underlying meaning in the expressed words). The results indicated that there was no difference between the genders in the manifest content. However, the females latently described their memories in more communal terms than males did, which supports the assumption that females are more communally oriented than are males.
  •  
Skapa referenser, mejla, bekava och länka
  • Resultat 1-10 av 178
Typ av publikation
tidskriftsartikel (89)
konferensbidrag (64)
bokkapitel (15)
annan publikation (3)
forskningsöversikt (3)
doktorsavhandling (2)
visa fler...
samlingsverk (redaktörskap) (1)
bok (1)
visa färre...
Typ av innehåll
refereegranskat (127)
övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt (50)
populärvet., debatt m.m. (1)
Författare/redaktör
Garcia, Danilo, 1973 (68)
Kjell, Oscar (19)
Cloninger, Kevin M. (15)
Rosenberg, Patricia, ... (13)
Kjell, Oscar N. E. (13)
visa fler...
Amato, Clara (12)
Nima, Ali Al (11)
Cloninger, C. Robert (8)
Gustafsson Sendén, M ... (8)
Garcia, Danilo (7)
Willander, Johan (7)
Kallioinen, Petter (7)
Johansson, Petter (6)
Hall, Lars (6)
Lindskär, Erik (6)
Granjard, Alexandre (6)
Archer, Trevor, 1949 (5)
Lindholm, Torun (5)
Sarwar, Farhan (5)
Hosseinian, Simin (4)
Rapp-Ricciardi, Max, ... (4)
Tärning, Betty (4)
Karlsson, Kristina (4)
Dahl, Mats (4)
Gustafsson Sendén, M ... (4)
Allwood, Carl Martin (3)
Anckarsäter, Henrik, ... (3)
Innes-Ker, Åse (3)
Mihailovic, Marko (3)
Kullberg, Jonas (3)
Bååth, Rasmus (3)
Daukantaitė, Daiva (3)
Fredén, Annika, 1981 ... (3)
Drejing, Karl (3)
Persson, Björn N. (3)
Bäck, Hanna (2)
Nilsson, Lars-Göran (2)
Bäck, Emma (2)
Lind, Andreas (2)
Dahlman, Christian (2)
Jönsson, Fredrik U. (2)
Wahlberg, Lena (2)
Söderlund, Göran, 19 ... (2)
Sczesny, Sabine (2)
Li, Shu-Chen (2)
Jönsson, Fredrik (2)
Kullving, Marie (2)
Molander-Söderholm, ... (2)
Rappe, Catrin (2)
visa färre...
Lärosäte
Lunds universitet (123)
Göteborgs universitet (72)
Stockholms universitet (26)
Södertörns högskola (6)
Linköpings universitet (5)
Högskolan i Gävle (4)
visa fler...
Uppsala universitet (3)
Karlstads universitet (3)
Högskolan Kristianstad (2)
Umeå universitet (1)
Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan (1)
Mälardalens universitet (1)
Handelshögskolan i Stockholm (1)
Mittuniversitetet (1)
Linnéuniversitetet (1)
Karolinska Institutet (1)
visa färre...
Språk
Engelska (174)
Svenska (4)
Forskningsämne (UKÄ/SCB)
Samhällsvetenskap (178)
Humaniora (18)
Medicin och hälsovetenskap (12)
Naturvetenskap (4)
Lantbruksvetenskap (1)

År

Kungliga biblioteket hanterar dina personuppgifter i enlighet med EU:s dataskyddsförordning (2018), GDPR. Läs mer om hur det funkar här.
Så här hanterar KB dina uppgifter vid användning av denna tjänst.

 
pil uppåt Stäng

Kopiera och spara länken för att återkomma till aktuell vy