SwePub
Sök i SwePub databas

  Utökad sökning

Träfflista för sökning "LAR1:gu ;lar1:(lu);lar1:(sh)"

Sökning: LAR1:gu > Lunds universitet > Södertörns högskola

  • Resultat 1-10 av 12
Sortera/gruppera träfflistan
   
NumreringReferensOmslagsbildHitta
1.
  •  
2.
  • Boström, Magnus, 1972-, et al. (författare)
  • Social sustainability requires social sustainability procedural prerequisites for reaching substantive goals
  • 2015
  • Ingår i: Nature and Culture. - : Berghahn Books. - 1558-6073 .- 1558-5468. ; 10:2, s. 131-156
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • The synergies and trade-offs between the various dimensions of sustainable development are attracting a rising scholarly attention. Departing from the scholarly debate, this article focuses on internal relationships within social sustainability. Our key claim is that it is diffi cult to strengthen substantive social sustainability goals unless there are key elements of social sustainability contained in the very procedures intended to work toward sustainability. Our analysis, informed by an organizing perspective, is based on a set of case studies on multi-stakeholder transnational sustainability projects (sustainability standards). This article explores six challenges related to the achievement of such procedures that can facilitate substantive social sustainability. Three of these concern the formulation of standards and policies, and three the implementation of standards and policies. To achieve substantive social sustainability procedures must be set in motion with abilities to take hold of people's concerns, frames, resources, as well as existing relevant institutions and infrastructures.
  •  
3.
  • Bäck, Emma, et al. (författare)
  • From I to We : Group Formation and Linguistic Adaption in an Online Xenophobic Forum
  • 2018
  • Ingår i: The Journal of Social and Political Psychology. - : Leibniz Institute for Psychology (ZPID). - 2195-3325. ; 6:1, s. 76-91
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Much of identity formation processes nowadays takes place online, indicating that intergroup differentiation may be found in online communities. This paper focuses on identity formation processes in an open online xenophobic, anti-immigrant, discussion forum. Open discussion forums provide an excellent opportunity to investigate open interactions that may reveal how identity is formed and how individual users are influenced by other users. Using computational text analysis and Linguistic Inquiry Word Count (LIWC), our results show that new users change from an individual identification to a group identification over time as indicated by a decrease in the use of “I” and increase in the use of “we”. The analyses also show increased use of “they” indicating intergroup differentiation. Moreover, the linguistic style of new users became more similar to that of the overall forum over time. Further, the emotional content decreased over time. The results indicate that new users on a forum create a collective identity with the other users and adapt to them linguistically.
  •  
4.
  •  
5.
  • Gustafsson Sendén, Marie, et al. (författare)
  • Introducing a gender-neutral pronoun in a natural gender language: the influence of time on attitudes and behavior
  • 2015
  • Ingår i: Frontiers in Psychology. - : Frontiers Media SA. - 1664-1078. ; 6
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • The implementation of gender fair language is often associated with negative reactions and hostile attacks on people who propose a change. This was also the case in Sweden in 2012 when a third gender-neutral pronoun hen was proposed as an addition to the already existing Swedish pronouns for she (hon) and he (han). The pronoun hen can be used both generically, when gender is unknown or irrelevant, and as a transgender pronoun for people who categorize themselves outside the gender dichotomy. In this article we review the process from 2012 to 2015. No other language has so far added a third gender-neutral pronoun, existing parallel with two gendered pronouns, that actually have reached the broader population of language users. This makes the situation in Sweden unique. We present data on attitudes toward hen during the past 4 years and analyze how time is associated with the attitudes in the process of introducing hen to the Swedish language. In 2012 the majority of the Swedish population was negative to the word, but already in 2014 there was a significant shift to more positive attitudes. Time was one of the strongest predictors for attitudes also when other relevant factors were controlled for. The actual use of the word also increased, although to a lesser extent than the attitudes shifted. We conclude that new words challenging the binary gender system evoke hostile and negative reactions, but also that attitudes can normalize rather quickly. We see this finding very positive and hope it could motivate language amendments and initiatives for gender-fair language, although the first responses may be negative.
  •  
6.
  • Gustafsson Sendén, Marie, et al. (författare)
  • The (Not So) Changing Man: Dynamic Gender Stereotypes in Sweden
  • 2019
  • Ingår i: Frontiers in Psychology. - : Frontiers Media SA. - 1664-1078. ; 10
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • According to Social Role Theory, gender stereotypes are dynamic constructs influenced by actual and perceived changes in what roles women and men occupy (Wood and Eagly, 2011). Sweden is ranked as one of the most egalitarian countries in the world, with a strong national equality discourse and a relatively high number of men engaging in traditionally communal roles such as parenting and domestic tasks. This would imply a perceived change toward higher communion among men. Therefore, we investigated the dynamics of gender stereotype content in Sweden with a primary interest in the male stereotype and perceptions of gender equality. In Study 1, participants (N = 323) estimated descriptive stereotype content of women and men in Sweden in the past, present, or future. They also estimated gender distribution in occupations and domestic roles for each time-point. Results showed that the female stereotype increased in agentic traits from the past to the present, whereas the male stereotype showed no change in either agentic or communal traits. Furthermore, participants estimated no change in gender stereotypes for the future, and they overestimated how often women and men occupy gender non-traditional roles at present. In Study 2, we controlled for participants' actual knowledge about role change by either describing women's increased responsibilities on the job market, or men's increased responsibility at home (or provided no description). Participants (N = 648) were randomized to the three different conditions. Overall, women were perceived to increase in agentic traits, and this change was mediated by perceptions of social role occupation. Men where not perceived to increase in communion but decreased in agency when change focused on women's increased participation in the labor market. These results indicate that role change among women also influence perceptions of the male stereotype. Altogether, the results indicate that social roles might have stronger influence on perceptions of agency than perceptions of communion, and that communion could be harder to incorporate in the male stereotype.
  •  
7.
  •  
8.
  •  
9.
  • Lindqvist, Anna, et al. (författare)
  • Reducing a Male Bias in Language? : Establishing the Efficiency of Three Different Gender-Fair Language Strategies
  • 2019
  • Ingår i: Sex Roles. - : Springer. - 0360-0025 .- 1573-2762. ; 1:1-2, s. 109-117
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Different strategies of gender-fair language have been applied to reduce a male bias, which means the implicit belief that a word describing an undefined person describes a man. This male bias might be caused by the words themselves in terms of generic masculine or masculine forms or by androcentrism (the conflation of men with humanity). In two experiments, we tested how different gender-fair strategies used as labels of an unknown social target (an applicant in a recruitment situation) could eliminate the male bias. The three types of gender-fair strategies tested were: (a) paired forms (he/she), (b) traditional neutral words (e.g., singular they, “the applicant”), or (c) gender-neutral third-person pronouns actively created to challenge the binary gender system (ze, Swedish hen). The two experiments were performed in Swedish with 417 undergraduates in Sweden and in English with 411 U.S. participants recruited online. In Swedish, the third-person gender-neutral pronoun singular (hen) was used. In English, several forms of such gender-neutral pronouns have been suggested (e.g., ze). In both experiments, results indicated that paired forms and actively created gender-neutral pronouns eliminated the male bias, whereas traditional neutral words contained a male bias. Thus, gender-fair language strategies should avoid using traditional words. Consequences of using paired forms and creating new gender-neutral words are discussed. We argue that an actively created gender-neutral pronoun is of highest value because it is more inclusive. © 2018, The Author(s).
  •  
10.
  •  
Skapa referenser, mejla, bekava och länka
  • Resultat 1-10 av 12
Typ av publikation
tidskriftsartikel (9)
samlingsverk (redaktörskap) (1)
rapport (1)
bok (1)
Typ av innehåll
refereegranskat (7)
populärvet., debatt m.m. (3)
övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt (2)
Författare/redaktör
Jiang, Y. (1)
Liu, Y. (1)
Krueger, H. (1)
Zhao, Y. (1)
Allen, K. (1)
Gupta, R. (1)
visa fler...
Kim, S. (1)
Gupta, V. (1)
Kim, D. (1)
Leinsalu, Mall (1)
Weiderpass, E (1)
Sörensen, Jens (1)
Kivipelto, M (1)
Madison, Guy (1)
Johansson, Håkan (1)
Larsson, Anders (1)
Tonelli, M. (1)
Molina, Irene (1)
Wang, L (1)
Yu, C. (1)
Casey, D. (1)
Uthman, OA (1)
Abbafati, C (1)
Abbas, KM (1)
Abd-Allah, F (1)
Abu-Raddad, LJ (1)
Adedeji, IA (1)
Afshin, A (1)
Agrawal, A (1)
Ahmadieh, H (1)
Akinyemi, RO (1)
Al-Aly, Z (1)
Alam, K (1)
Alla, F (1)
Allebeck, P (1)
Altirkawi, KA (1)
Alvis-Guzman, N (1)
Amare, AT (1)
Antonio, CAT (1)
Arnlov, J (1)
Avokpaho, EFGA (1)
Quintanilla, BPA (1)
Badawi, A (1)
Barker-Collo, SL (1)
Barrero, LH (1)
Basu, S (1)
Bedi, N (1)
Beghi, E (1)
Bejot, Y (1)
Bello, AK (1)
visa färre...
Lärosäte
Göteborgs universitet (12)
Stockholms universitet (5)
Uppsala universitet (2)
Örebro universitet (2)
visa fler...
Mälardalens universitet (1)
Handelshögskolan i Stockholm (1)
Linnéuniversitetet (1)
Karolinska Institutet (1)
Högskolan Dalarna (1)
visa färre...
Språk
Engelska (8)
Svenska (4)
Forskningsämne (UKÄ/SCB)
Samhällsvetenskap (10)
Humaniora (3)
Naturvetenskap (1)
Medicin och hälsovetenskap (1)

År

Kungliga biblioteket hanterar dina personuppgifter i enlighet med EU:s dataskyddsförordning (2018), GDPR. Läs mer om hur det funkar här.
Så här hanterar KB dina uppgifter vid användning av denna tjänst.

 
pil uppåt Stäng

Kopiera och spara länken för att återkomma till aktuell vy