SwePub
Tyck till om SwePub Sök här!
Sök i SwePub databas

  Utökad sökning

Träfflista för sökning "WFRF:(Gonzalez Bellido Paloma) "

Sökning: WFRF:(Gonzalez Bellido Paloma)

  • Resultat 1-7 av 7
Sortera/gruppera träfflistan
   
NumreringReferensOmslagsbildHitta
1.
  • Bernal, Ximena E., et al. (författare)
  • Empowering Latina scientists
  • 2019
  • Ingår i: Science. - : American Association for the Advancement of Science (AAAS). - 0036-8075 .- 1095-9203. ; 363:6429, s. 825-826
  • Tidskriftsartikel (övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt)
  •  
2.
  • Gonzalez-Bellido, Paloma T, et al. (författare)
  • Target detection in insects : optical, neural and behavioral optimizations
  • 2016
  • Ingår i: Current Opinion in Neurobiology. - : Elsevier BV. - 0959-4388 .- 1873-6882. ; 41, s. 122-128
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Motion vision provides important cues for many tasks. Flying insects, for example, may pursue small, fast moving targets for mating or feeding purposes, even when these are detected against self-generated optic flow. Since insects are small, with size-constrained eyes and brains, they have evolved to optimize their optical, neural and behavioral target visualization solutions. Indeed, even if evolutionarily distant insects display different pursuit strategies, target neuron physiology is strikingly similar. Furthermore, the coarse spatial resolution of the insect compound eye might actually be beneficial when it comes to detection of moving targets. In conclusion, tiny insects show higher than expected performance in target visualization tasks.
  •  
3.
  • Nicholas, Sarah, et al. (författare)
  • Integration of Small- and Wide-Field Visual Features in Target-Selective Descending Neurons of both Predatory and Non-Predatory Dipterans
  • 2018
  • Ingår i: Journal of Neuroscience. - 0270-6474 .- 1529-2401. ; 38:50, s. 10725-10733
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • For many animals, target motion carries high ecological significance as this may be generated by a predator, prey or potential mate. Indeed, animals whose survival depends on early target detection are often equipped with a sharply tuned visual system, yielding robust performance in challenging conditions. For example, many fast-flying insects use visual cues for identifying targets, such as prey (e.g. predatory dragonflies and robberflies) or conspecifics (e.g. non-predatory hoverflies), and can often do so against self-generated background optic flow. Supporting these behaviors, the optic lobes of insects that pursue targets harbor neurons that respond robustly to the motion of small moving objects, even when displayed against syn-directional background clutter. However, in diptera, the encoding of target information by the descending neurons, which are more directly involved in generating the behavioral output, has received less attention. We characterized target selective neurons by recording in the ventral nerve cord of male and female predatory Holcocephala fusca robberflies and of male non-predatory Eristalis tenax hoverflies. We show that both species have dipteran target-selective descending neurons (dTSDNs) that only respond to target motion if the background is stationary or moving slowly, moves in the opposite direction, or has un-naturalistic spatial characteristics. The response to the target is suppressed when background and target move at similar velocities, which is strikingly different to the response of target neurons in the optic lobes. As the neurons we recorded from are pre-motor, our findings affect our interpretation of the neurophysiology underlying target-tracking behaviors.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENTMany animals use sensory cues to detect moving targets that may represent predators, prey or conspecifics. For example, birds of prey show superb sensitivity to the motion of small prey, and intercept these at high speeds. In a similar manner, predatory insects visually track moving prey, often against cluttered backgrounds. Accompanying this behavior, the brains of insects that pursue targets contain neurons that respond exclusively to target motion. We here show that dipteran insects also have target selective descending neurons in the part of their nervous system that corresponds to the vertebrate spinal cord. Surprisingly, and in contrast to the neurons in the brain, these pre-motor neurons are inhibited by background patterns moving in the same direction as the target.
  •  
4.
  • Nordström, Karin, 1973-, et al. (författare)
  • Invertebrate vision : Peripheral adaptation to repeated object motion
  • 2013
  • Ingår i: Current Biology. - : Cell Press. - 0960-9822 .- 1879-0445. ; 23:15, s. R655-R656
  • Tidskriftsartikel (övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt)abstract
    • Visual systems adapt rapidly to objects moving repeatedly within the visual field, because such objects are likely irrelevant. In the crab, the neural switch for such adaptation has been found to take place at a surprisingly early stage of the visual processing pathway.
  •  
5.
  • Thyselius, Malin, 1990-, et al. (författare)
  • Hoverfly (Eristalis tenax) pursuit of artificial targets
  • 2023
  • Ingår i: Journal of Experimental Biology. - : The Company of Biologists. - 0022-0949 .- 1477-9145. ; 226:4
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • The ability to visualize small moving objects is vital for the survival of many animals, as these could represent predators or prey. For example, predatory insects, including dragonflies, robber flies and killer flies, perform elegant, high-speed pursuits of both biological and artificial targets. Many non-predatory insects, including male hoverflies and blowflies, also pursue targets during territorial or courtship interactions. To date, most hoverfly pursuits have been studied outdoors. To investigate hoverfly (Eristalis tenax) pursuits under more controlled settings, we constructed an indoor arena that was large enough to encourage naturalistic behavior. We presented artificial beads of different sizes, moving at different speeds, and filmed pursuits with two cameras, allowing subsequent 3D reconstruction of the hoverfly and bead position as a function of time. We show that male E. tenax hoverflies are unlikely to use strict heuristic rules based on angular size or speed to determine when to start pursuit, at least in our indoor setting. We found that hoverflies pursued faster beads when the trajectory involved flying downwards towards the bead. Furthermore, we show that target pursuit behavior can be broken down into two stages. In the first stage, the hoverfly attempts to rapidly decreases the distance to the target by intercepting it at high speed. During the second stage, the hoverfly's forward speed is correlated with the speed of the bead, so that the hoverfly remains close, but without catching it. This may be similar to dragonfly shadowing behavior, previously coined ‘motion camouflage’.
  •  
6.
  • Thyselius, Malin, et al. (författare)
  • Visual approach computation in feeding hoverflies
  • 2018
  • Ingår i: Journal of Experimental Biology. - : The Company of Biologists. - 0022-0949 .- 1477-9145. ; 221:10
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • On warm sunny days, female hoverflies are often observed feeding from a wide range of wild and cultivated flowers. In doing so, hoverflies serve a vital role as alternative pollinators, and are suggested to be the most important pollinators after bees and bumblebees. Unless the flower hoverflies are feeding from is large, they do not readily share the space with other insects, but instead opt to leave if another insect approaches. We used high-speed videography followed by 3D reconstruction of flight trajectories to quantify how female Eristalis hoverflies respond to approaching bees, wasps and two different hoverfly species. We found that, in 94% of the interactions, the occupant female left the flower when approached by another insect. We found that compared with spontaneous take-offs, the occupant hoverfly's escape response was performed at similar to 3 times higher speed (spontaneous take-off at 0.20.05 m s(-1) compared with 0.55 +/- 0.08 m s(-1) when approached by another Eristalis). The hoverflies tended to take off upward and forward, while taking the incomer's approach angle into account. Intriguingly, we found that, when approached by wasps, the occupant Eristalis took off at a higher speed and when the wasp was further away. This suggests that feeding hoverflies may be able to distinguish these predators, demanding impressive visual capabilities. Our results, including quantification of the visual information available before occupant take-off, provide important insight into how freely behaving hoverflies perform escape responses from competitors and predators (e.g. wasps) in the wild.
  •  
7.
  • Wardill, Trevor J, et al. (författare)
  • A Novel Interception Strategy in a Miniature Robber Fly with Extreme Visual Acuity
  • 2017
  • Ingår i: Current Biology. - : Elsevier BV. - 0960-9822 .- 1879-0445. ; 27:6, s. 854-859
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Our visual system allows us to rapidly identify and intercept a moving object. When this object is far away, we base the trajectory on the target's location relative to an external frame of reference [1]. This process forms the basis for the constant bearing angle (CBA) model, a reactive strategy that ensures interception since the bearing angle, formed between the line joining pursuer and target (called the range vector) and an external reference line, is held constant [2-4]. The CBA model may be a fundamental and widespread strategy, as it is also known to explain the interception trajectories of bats and fish [5, 6]. Here, we show that the aerial attack of the tiny robber fly Holcocephala fusca is consistent with the CBA model. In addition, Holcocephala fusca displays a novel proactive strategy, termed "lock-on" phase, embedded with the later part of the flight. We found the object detection threshold for this species to be 0.13°, enabled by an extremely specialized, forward pointing fovea (∼5 ommatidia wide, interommatidial angle Δφ = 0.28°, photoreceptor acceptance angle Δρ = 0.27°). This study furthers our understanding of the accurate performance that a miniature brain can achieve in highly demanding sensorimotor tasks and suggests the presence of equivalent mechanisms for target interception across a wide range of taxa.
  •  
Skapa referenser, mejla, bekava och länka
  • Resultat 1-7 av 7
Typ av publikation
tidskriftsartikel (7)
Typ av innehåll
refereegranskat (5)
övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt (2)
Författare/redaktör
Nordström, Karin, 19 ... (5)
Nordström, Karin (1)
Castaneda-Gomez, Lau ... (1)
Guedes, Thais (1)
Zapata, Felipe (1)
Orizaola, German (1)
visa fler...
Dick, Christopher W. (1)
Pimiento, Catalina (1)
Ortiz-Barrientos, Da ... (1)
Bernal, Ximena E. (1)
Rojas, Bibiana (1)
Pinto-E, Maria Aleja ... (1)
Mendoza-Henao, Angel ... (1)
Herrera-Montes, Adri ... (1)
Isabel Herrera-Monte ... (1)
Caceres Franco, Andr ... (1)
Ceron-Souza, Ivania (1)
Paz, Andrea (1)
Vergara, Daniela (1)
Barragan Contreras, ... (1)
Salazar, Camilo (1)
Bohorquez Alonso, Ma ... (1)
Sanchez, Adriana (1)
Olaya-Castro, Alexan ... (1)
Urbina-Cardona, Nico ... (1)
Guayasamin, Juan M. (1)
Uy, Floria Mora-Kepf ... (1)
Feris, Faride Lamadr ... (1)
Franco-Orozco, Barba ... (1)
Munoz, Martha M. (1)
Patricia Rincon-Diaz ... (1)
Sanchez Herrera, Mel ... (1)
Betancourth-Cundar, ... (1)
Tarvin, Rebecca D. (1)
Marquez, Roberto (1)
Lopez-Aguirre, Camil ... (1)
Ron, Santiago R. (1)
Ramirez, Santiago (1)
Paez-Vacas, Monica (1)
Gaitan-Espitia, Juan ... (1)
Vianna, Juliana A. (1)
Varela-Jaramillo, An ... (1)
Sanchez-Martinez, Pa ... (1)
Caminer Rodriguez, M ... (1)
Garcia-Robledo, Carl ... (1)
Kuprewicz, Erin K. (1)
Gomez-Bahamon, Valen ... (1)
Chacon-Vargas, Kathe ... (1)
Trillo, Alex (1)
Ramirez Castaneda, V ... (1)
visa färre...
Lärosäte
Uppsala universitet (6)
Chalmers tekniska högskola (1)
Språk
Engelska (7)
Forskningsämne (UKÄ/SCB)
Naturvetenskap (4)
Medicin och hälsovetenskap (4)

År

Kungliga biblioteket hanterar dina personuppgifter i enlighet med EU:s dataskyddsförordning (2018), GDPR. Läs mer om hur det funkar här.
Så här hanterar KB dina uppgifter vid användning av denna tjänst.

 
pil uppåt Stäng

Kopiera och spara länken för att återkomma till aktuell vy