SwePub
Tyck till om SwePub Sök här!
Sök i SwePub databas

  Utökad sökning

Träfflista för sökning "WFRF:(Nilsson Lars Göran) ;pers:(Rönnlund Michael)"

Sökning: WFRF:(Nilsson Lars Göran) > Rönnlund Michael

  • Resultat 1-10 av 33
Sortera/gruppera träfflistan
   
NumreringReferensOmslagsbildHitta
1.
  • Lövdén, Martin, et al. (författare)
  • The extent of stability and change in episodic and semantic memory in old age : Demographic predictors of level and change
  • 2004
  • Ingår i: The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences. - Washington, DC : Gerontological Society of America. - 1079-5014 .- 1758-5368. ; 59:3, s. 130-134
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Structural stability and change in semantic and episodic memory performance as well as interindividual differences in 5-year changes in these constructs are examined within a sample of older adults (age rangeT1 = 60–80; n = 361). Interindividual differences in change were limited but significant. Stability coefficients were higher for semantic memory (.95) than for episodic memory (.87). Changes in episodic and semantic memory performance were strongly associated (r =.68). Across time, variances and covariances increased, and a tendency toward dedifferentiation in terms of increasing correlations was found. Chronological age was related to both level and change, but gender and education were only related to level of memory performance. Collectively, these results depict relatively high degrees of structural stability and stability of interindividual differences in declarative memory in old age.  
  •  
2.
  • Rönnlund, Michael, et al. (författare)
  • Stability, Growth, and Decline in Adult Life Span Development of Declarative Memory : Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Data From a Population-Based Study
  • 2005
  • Ingår i: Psychology and Aging. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0882-7974 .- 1939-1498. ; 20:1, s. 3-18
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Five-year changes in episodic and semantic memory were examined in a sample of 829 participants (35-80 years). A cohort-matched sample (N=967) was assessed to control for practice effects. For episodic memory, cross-sectional analyses indicated gradual age-related decrements, whereas the longitudinal data revealed no decrements before age 60, even when practice effects were adjusted for. Longitudinally, semantic memory showed minor increments until age 55, with smaller decrements in old age as compared with episodic memory. Cohort differences in educational attainment appear to account for the discrepancies between cross-sectional and longitudinal data. Collectively, the results show that age trajectories for episodic and semantic memory differ and underscore the need to control for cohort and retest effects in cross-sectional and longitudinal studies, respectively.
  •  
3.
  • Nilsson, Lars-Göran, 1944-, et al. (författare)
  • Challenging the notion of an early-onset of cognitive decline.
  • 2009
  • Ingår i: Neurobiology of Aging. - : Elsevier. - 0197-4580 .- 1558-1497. ; 30:4, s. 521-524; discussion 530
  • Tidskriftsartikel (övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt)abstract
    • Salthouse claims that cognitive aging starts around 20 years of age. The basis for this claim is cross-sectional data. He dismisses longitudinal data, which typically show the cognitive decline to start much later, around 60 years of age. He states that longitudinal data cannot be trusted because they are flawed. There is a confounding between the effects of maturation and retest effects. We challenge Salthouse's strong claim on four accounts.
  •  
4.
  •  
5.
  • Olofsson, Jonas K., et al. (författare)
  • Odor Identification Deficit as a Predictor of Five-Year Global Cognitive Change : Interactive Effects with Age and ApoE-ε4
  • 2009
  • Ingår i: Behavior Genetics. - : Springer Netherlands. - 0001-8244 .- 1573-3297. ; 39:5, s. 496-503
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Olfactory impairments are present in common neurodegenerative disorders and predict conversion to dementia in non-demented individuals with cognitive impairment. In cognitively intact elderly, evidence is sparse regarding the role of olfactory deficits in predicting cognitive impairment. The present study investigated predictors of 5-year prospective decline in the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) in a large (n = 501), population-based sample of elderly (65–90 years) individuals. All participants were genotyped for the ApoE gene, assessed for health factors, and were non-demented at the baseline assessment. After partialling out the influences of demographic and health-factors at baseline and dementia at follow-up, poor odor identification ability in combination with older age and the ApoE-ε4 allele predicted larger prospective global cognitive decline. This effect could not be produced by a vocabulary test. In sum, the findings suggest that an olfactory deficit can dissociate between benign and malign global cognitive development in non-demented, very old ε4-carriers, who are at high risk of developing dementia.
  •  
6.
  • Rönnlund, Michael, et al. (författare)
  • Secular trends in cognitive test performance : Swedish conscript data 1970–1993
  • 2013
  • Ingår i: Intelligence. - : Elsevier BV. - 0160-2896 .- 1873-7935. ; 41:1, s. 19-24
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • We investigated time-related patterns in levels of cognitive performance during the period from 1970 to 1993 based on data from Swedish draft boards. The conscripts, including more than a million 18–19-year old men, had taken one of two versions of the Swedish enlistment battery (SEB67; 1970–1979 or SEB80; 1980–1993), each composed of four subtests. The results revealed significant Flynn effects, with estimated gains of 1.2–1.5 IQ-units per decade. The effect seem to hold across ability levels, even though tendencies of more pronounced effects in the lower half of the ability distribution was observed. The largest gains were for visuospatial tests (Paper Form Board and Metal Folding), with little change, even slight losses during the second sub-period, for tests of verbal knowledge (Concept Discrimination and Synonyms) and a mixed pattern for a test of technical comprehension (losses followed by gains). Finally, comparisons of trends in cognitive performance and in standing height show that the gains in cognitive performance over the years from 1980 to 1993 occurred in the absence of overall gains in height, which speaks against nutrition as the cause of the Flynn effects.
  •  
7.
  • Vestergren, Peter, 1974-, et al. (författare)
  • Development of the cognitive dysfunction questionnaire (CDQ) in a population based sample
  • 2011
  • Ingår i: Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. - Stockholm : Almqvist & Wiksell. - 0036-5564 .- 1467-9450. ; 52:3, s. 218-228
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • The study reports on the development of a questionnaire for assessment of adult cognitive dysfunction (CDQ). Participants in a population-based sample(65 ± 15 years, N = 370) responded to a 90-item pilot version covering multiple aspects of memory/cognition. Based on exploratory principal components analyses and correlations with criterion measures of cognitive functioning (MMSE, Block Design, semantic/episodic memory), 20 items loading on 6 components were selected for the final version of the questionnaire. Cronbach’s a for the total score was 0.90. There was evidence of construct validity as judged by correlations between CDQ scores, objective cognitive measures, and a subjective memory measure (PRMQ). Discriminant validity was demonstrated by a low and non-significant correlation with depressive symptoms. Further evidence of construct validity was provided by correlations with age and educational attainment. In conclusion, the CDQ is promising as a self-rating screening tool for cognitive dysfunction, and will be the subject of further development and validation.
  •  
8.
  • Vestergren, Peter, 1974-, et al. (författare)
  • Multigroup confirmatory factor analysis of the cognitive dysfunction questionnaire : instrument refinement and measurement invariance across age and sex
  • 2012
  • Ingår i: Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. - : Wiley-Blackwell. - 0036-5564 .- 1467-9450. ; 53:5, s. 390-400
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • The study adopted CFA to investigate the factorial structure and reduce the number of items of the Cognitive Dysfunction Questionnaire (CDQ; Vestergren, Rönnlund, Nyberg, & Nilsson, 2011). The analyses were based on data for a total of 1115 participants from population based samples (mean age: 63.0 ± 14.5 years, range: 25 - 95) randomly split into a refinement (n = 569) and a cross-validation (n = 546) sample. Equivalence of the measurement and structural portions of the refined model was demonstrated across the refinement and cross-validation samples. Among competing models the best fitting and parsimonious model had a hierarchical factor structure with five first-order and one second-order general factor. The final version of the CDQ consisted of 20 items in five domains (Procedural actions, Semantic word knowledge, Face recognition, Temporal orientation and Spatial navigation). Internal consistency reliabilities were adequate for the total scale and for the subscales. Multigroup CFAs were performed and the results indicate measurement invariance across age and sex up to the scalar level. Finally, higher levels of cognitive dysfunction as reflected by CDQ scores were observed with advancing age and with deficits in general cognitive functioning as reflected by scores on the Mini-Mental State Examination. In conclusion, adoption of the final version of the CDQ appears to be a way of measuring cognitive dysfunction without administering formal cognitive tests. Future studies should apply it among clinical groups to further test its usefulness.
  •  
9.
  • Eriksson, Daniel, et al. (författare)
  • Effects of perceived long-term stress on health and memory functioning
  • 2010
  • Ingår i: Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society. ; , s. 78-78
  • Konferensbidrag (refereegranskat)abstract
    • The study examined effects of perceived long-term stress on health and memory functioning in middle-aged individuals (40–60 years). Participants in the Betula study (Nilsson et al., 1997) describing themselves as being stressed in general over three measurement occasions (10 years in total) were compared with a matched (sex and education) group (n = 98) reporting no stress. The results revealed a higher incidence of depressive symptoms, flus, and not-healthy-ratings over time for the stress group. In addition, the stress group provided more negative subjective memory ratings, whereas time-related change in memory performance, indicative of a high degree of cognitive stability, did not differ from that of controls. Degree of perceived stress is discussed as a factor underlying variations in regard to the outcome of studies of perceived stress.
  •  
10.
  • Eriksson Sörman, Daniel, et al. (författare)
  • Leisure Activity in Old Age and Risk of Dementia : a 15-Year Prospective Study
  • 2014
  • Ingår i: The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences. - : Oxford University Press. - 1079-5014 .- 1758-5368. ; 69:4, s. 493-501
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Objectives. The aim of this study was to investigate whether leisure activity is associated with incident dementia in an older sample.Method. We examined a sample of 1,475 elderly (>= 65 years) who were dementia free at baseline over a follow-up period of up to 15 years. In addition to analyses involving the total time period, separate analyses of three time periods were performed, 1-5, 6-10, and 11-15 years, following baseline measurement of leisure activity.Results. After controlling for a variety of potential confounders, analyses of data for the total time period revealed that higher levels of "Total activity" and "Social activity," but not "Mental activity," were associated with decreased risk of dementia. However, analyses of the separate time periods showed that this association was only significant in the first time period, 1-5 years after baseline.Discussion. The results from this study provide little support for the hypothesis that frequent engagement in leisure activities among elderly serve to protect against dementia diseases across a longer time frame. The finding of a relationship for the first time period, 1-5 years after baseline, could indicate short-term protective effects but could also reflect reverse causality.
  •  
Skapa referenser, mejla, bekava och länka
  • Resultat 1-10 av 33
Typ av publikation
tidskriftsartikel (30)
konferensbidrag (2)
doktorsavhandling (1)
Typ av innehåll
refereegranskat (30)
övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt (3)
Författare/redaktör
Nilsson, Lars-Göran (32)
Sundström, Anna (8)
Rönnlund, Michael, 1 ... (7)
Adolfsson, Rolf (5)
Lövdén, Martin (5)
visa fler...
Nyberg, Lars (4)
Nyberg, Lars, 1966- (3)
Eriksson Sörman, Dan ... (3)
Vestergren, Peter, 1 ... (3)
Mäntylä, Timo, 1954- (2)
Eriksson, Daniel (1)
Kormi-Nouri, Reza, 1 ... (1)
Backman, Lars (1)
Larsson, Maria (1)
Nordin, M. (1)
Wahlin, Åke (1)
Norberg, Margareta (1)
Bäckman, Lars (1)
Weinehall, Lars (1)
Nordin, Steven, 1960 ... (1)
Olofsson, Jonas K. (1)
Sternäng, Ola (1)
Juslin, Peter (1)
Bergdahl, Jan (1)
Bergdahl, Maud (1)
Blomstedt, Yulia (1)
Carlstedt, Berit (1)
Eriksson Sörman, Dan ... (1)
Stenling, Andreas (1)
Hansson, Patrik (1)
Nilsson, Lars-Göran, ... (1)
Körning-Ljungberg, J ... (1)
Rönnlund, Michael, P ... (1)
Sundström, Anna, PhD (1)
Lövdén, Martin, Prof ... (1)
Sundström, Anna, 196 ... (1)
Michael, Rönnlund, 1 ... (1)
Vestergren, Peter (1)
Sörman Eriksson, Dan ... (1)
visa färre...
Lärosäte
Umeå universitet (27)
Stockholms universitet (25)
Karolinska Institutet (8)
Luleå tekniska universitet (5)
Uppsala universitet (1)
Örebro universitet (1)
visa fler...
Försvarshögskolan (1)
visa färre...
Språk
Engelska (32)
Svenska (1)
Forskningsämne (UKÄ/SCB)
Samhällsvetenskap (25)
Medicin och hälsovetenskap (7)
Teknik (1)

År

Kungliga biblioteket hanterar dina personuppgifter i enlighet med EU:s dataskyddsförordning (2018), GDPR. Läs mer om hur det funkar här.
Så här hanterar KB dina uppgifter vid användning av denna tjänst.

 
pil uppåt Stäng

Kopiera och spara länken för att återkomma till aktuell vy