SwePub
Sök i SwePub databas

  Utökad sökning

Träfflista för sökning "L773:1355 3259 OR L773:2044 8333 "

Sökning: L773:1355 3259 OR L773:2044 8333

  • Resultat 1-10 av 29
Sortera/gruppera träfflistan
   
NumreringReferensOmslagsbildHitta
1.
  • Archer, John, et al. (författare)
  • Perpetrator programmes for partner violence : A rejoinder to Respect
  • 2012
  • Ingår i: Legal and Criminological Psychology. - : Wiley. - 1355-3259 .- 2044-8333. ; 17:2, s. 225-232
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Purpose. To reply to the comments made by Debbonaire and Todd (2012) in relation to our critique of Respect's Position Statement. Method. We examined their reply in relation to our original article and to the wider research literature. Results. We show that Debbonaire and Todd's reply is largely a series of assertions, for which little or no supporting evidence is offered. Their argument is first that we are misplaced in criticizing their Position Statement, and second that the main points of the statement are defendable. We indicate why our criticisms of the statement still stand. Conclusions. We argue that Respect have not countered our overall criticism of their position that intimate partner violence (IPV) can only be addressed as a gendered issue, that is as a consequence of patriarchal values enacted at the individual level. Instead we advocate a gender-inclusive approach applying a knowledge base derived from robust empirical research on IPV and more widely from research on human aggression.
  •  
2.
  • Granhag, Pär Anders, et al. (författare)
  • Eliciting cues to deception by tactical disclosure of evidence : The first test of the Evidence Framing Matrix
  • 2012
  • Ingår i: Legal and Criminological Psychology. - : Wiley-Blackwell. - 1355-3259 .- 2044-8333. ; 18:2, s. 341-355
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Purpose. Research on real-life suspect interviews shows that disclosure of evidenceis a very common tactic and that it occurs in all phases of the interview. It istherefore remarkable that there is hardly any research on the effectiveness of differentdisclosure tactics. The aim of this study was to examine the effects of three differentdisclosure tactics: presenting the evidence early and two versions of the Strategic Useof Evidence (SUE) technique.Methods. For the SUE-Basic technique (SUE-B), the evidence was disclosed late inthe interview. For the SUE-Incremental technique (SUE-I), we used a stepwise disclosuretactic derived from the so-called Evidence Framing Matrix. The tactic consists of revealingevidence of increasing strength and precision. A mock-theft scenario was employedwith 195 participants who were randomly allocated to one of six conditions: guilty orinnocent suspects were interviewed with one of the three techniques. Two measures ofinconsistency were used as dependent variables: statement-evidence inconsistency andthe newly developed within-statement inconsistency.Results. By interviewing with SUE-I, strong cues to deception were elicited, especiallyfor the statement-evidence inconsistency variable. For the SUE-B, significant but smallerdifferences between guilty and innocent suspects were obtained.Conclusions. We found that both when and how the evidence was disclosedmoderated the effectiveness of disclosure. With respect to when, it was more effectiveto disclose the evidence late (vs. early), and with respect to how, it was more effective todisclose the evidence in a stepwise (vs. direct) manner. The tactical aspects of evidencedisclosure are discussed.
  •  
3.
  • Roos Af Hjelmsäter, Emma, 1976, et al. (författare)
  • ‘Mapping’ deception in adolescents : Eliciting cues to deceit through an unanticipated spatial drawing task
  • 2014
  • Ingår i: Legal and Criminological Psychology. - : John Wiley & Sons. - 1355-3259 .- 2044-8333. ; 19:1, s. 179-188
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Purpose. In this experiment we examined whether an unanticipated spatial task couldincrease the differences between lying and truth telling groups of adolescents. In addition,we explored whether there are some elements of such a spatial task that elicit morediagnostic cues to deception than others.Methods. In groups of three, adolescents (N = 150, aged 13–14) either experienced(‘truth tellers’) or imagined (‘liars’) an event. In subsequent individual interviews, theadolescents were asked to provide both a general verbal description of the event (theanticipated task), and a spatial description by making marks on a sketch (the unanticipatedtask). Next, adults (N = 200) rated the degree of consistency between either the generaldescriptions or the spatial descriptions from the adolescents in each triad.Results. The differences between liars and truth tellers were larger for the spatialmarkings (the unanticipated task) than for the general verbal descriptions (the anticipatedtask). Importantly, as predicted, the difference between lying and truth-telling triads wasmost manifest for markings of salient (vs. non-salient) aspects of the event.Conclusions. The results suggests that (a) using spatial tasks may be a useful tool fordetecting deception in adolescents, but that (b) the assessment of credibility should onlydraw on the salient aspects of the unanticipated spatial task.
  •  
4.
  • Willén, Rebecca M., 1980, et al. (författare)
  • Offenders' uncoerced false confessions: A new application of statement analysis?
  • 2012
  • Ingår i: Legal and Criminological Psychology. - : Wiley. - 1355-3259 .- 2044-8333. ; 17:2, s. 346-359
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Purpose. Deception detection research has mainly studied denials and distortions given by students. This study examined true and false confessions as told by offenders. It was hypothesized that the statement analytic techniques Criteria-Based Content Analysis (CBCA) and Reality Monitoring (RM) would discriminate truths and lies. Methods. Truthful and deceptive confessions to crime were given by 30 offenders (both women and men) in a within-subject design. The participants were in prison at the time of data collection, and told the truth about a crime they had committed and been sentenced for. In addition, they made up a lie about a different crime after a few minutes of preparation. The transcribed statements were scored for CBCA and RM criteria. Results. Results showed that neither total CBCA nor total RM scores differentiated between lies and truths. Some individual CBCA criteria, however, showed differences: more self-deprecations and doubts about own testimony in the told lies, and more unexpected complications in the truths. Conclusions. The results are discussed in relation to statement analysis of offenders’ accounts, individual CBCA criteria, as well as the development of criminal experience and familiarity with the event and setting. Implications for triers of fact and suggestions for future research are considered.
  •  
5.
  • Belfrage, Henrik, et al. (författare)
  • Management of violent behaviour in the correctional system using qualified risk assessments.
  • 2004
  • Ingår i: Legal and Criminological Psychology. - : Wiley-Blackwell. - 1355-3259 .- 2044-8333. ; 9:1, s. 11-22
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Purpose. This study focused on whether institutional violence in a maximum-security correctional institution could be prevented using comprehensive risk assessments followed by adequate risk management. And, could this be shown by a decrease in risk factors for violence according to the HCR-20 Risk Assessment Scheme in the study group? Methods. Offenders with a history of violent criminality were subject to real-life assessments using the HCR-20 Risk Assessment Scheme. The assessments were followed by discussions with members of staff, in which risk management strategies were designed. Thus, the members of staff were fully aware of every inmate's personality characteristics (e.g. psychiatric diagnoses), what risk factors for violence they displayed, and how best to manage those risk factors. With the aim of evaluating the possible effects of our interventions, approximately one third of the study group was reassessed after a mean of 12 months. Results. The follow-up showed no significant decrease in important risk factors for violence in the study group. However, the number of violent incidents showed a remarkable decrease during the study period. Conclusions. Not being able to reduce important risk factors for violence does not necessarily mean that one cannot decrease the risk for, or the incidence of, violence. This study indicates that proper and adequate risk management, using the best protective factors available, can reduce violence even though important risk factors cannot be decreased. The study also supports the theoretical assumption that changes in risk factors are more possible in some populations (e.g. general psychiatric) than in others (e.g. correctional) depending on the nature of the study group and the risk factors that are at hand (e.g. dynamic vs. static). This seems to be important to bear in mind when performing evaluation research using risk assessment instruments.
  •  
6.
  • Ernberg, Emelie, 1989, et al. (författare)
  • Interviewing witnesses in a second language: A comparison of interpreter-assisted, unaided, and self-administered interviews
  • 2023
  • Ingår i: Legal and Criminological Psychology. - : Wiley. - 1355-3259 .- 2044-8333. ; 28:1, s. 60-73
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Purpose With increasing rates of migration worldwide, police are more likely than ever to interview witnesses who do not have the same first language as they do. We examined how to best approach this situation by comparing three different ways of conducting such interviews. Methods Native Arabic speakers (N = 128) living in Sweden witnessed a video of a mock crime and were allocated to one of three interview conditions: a face-to-face interview in Swedish (i.e. their second language), a face-to-face interview with an interpreter translating from Swedish to Arabic or an Arabic language Self-Administered Interview (c) (SAI). Results For total number of details reported, the no interpreter condition resulted in moderately fewer details being reported than the interpreter and SAI conditions. A similar trend was seen for correct details; however, these differences were not statistically significant. Participants in the SAI condition were somewhat less accurate in their reports compared with both the interpreter and no interpreter conditions. Conclusions If interviewing without an interpreter, there is minimal loss of reported detail when the witness speaks the interviewer's language at an intermediate level and the questions posed are few and simple. Moreover, provided that the witness has a sufficient level of literacy, administrating the SAI in the witness's native language can be an alternative for witnesses with no or limited verbal ability in the interviewer's language.
  •  
7.
  • Landström, Sara, 1980, et al. (författare)
  • Children’s testimony and the emotional victim effect
  • 2015
  • Ingår i: Legal and Criminological Psychology. - : Wiley. - 1355-3259 .- 2044-8333. ; 20:2, s. 365-383
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Purpose. Two experiments were conducted to examine the effects of (1) child victims’ emotional expression during testimony and (2) the camera perspective used to record the testimony, on judgements of credibility. Methods. Law students (N = 155 in Experiment 1; N = 86 in Experiment 2) watched a child harassment complainant provide a statement in an emotional or neutral manner, presented using different camera perspectives: balanced focus (i.e., a shot portraying an equal focus on the child complainant and the interviewer) versus picture-in-picture (PiP; i.e., a shot portraying only the child with an inset window depicting both the child and the interviewer in the corner of the screen) in Experiment 1 and PiP versus child focus (i.e., a shot depicting only the child) in Experiment 2. Results. Although no effect was found for camera perspective, the results provide support for an emotional victim effect (EVE); the child was perceived as more credible and truthful when communicating the statement in an emotional (vs. neutral) manner. Moreover, the results provide corroborating evidence for the assumption that the EVE rests on both cognitive (expectancy confirmation) and affective (compassion) mechanisms. Conclusions. These findings extend previous research by showing that the EVE and its underlying mechanisms apply to judgements of child complainants in the context of non sexual crimes and appear to be robust against variations of camera perspectives. Legal implications are discussed.
  •  
8.
  • Lindholm, Torun (författare)
  • Validity in judgments of high- and low-accurate witnesses of own and other ethnic groups
  • 2008
  • Ingår i: Legal and Criminological Psychology. - 1355-3259 .- 2044-8333. ; 13:1, s. 107-121
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Purpose. Research has shown that people often have difficulties estimating eyewitness accuracy correctly. In most previous studies examining validity in credibility judgments, participants have assessed the accuracy of witnesses who have been homogeneous in their memory performance. This study investigated validity in judgments of witnesses who varied widely in memory. A further purpose was to examine whether judgmental validity was moderated by the witnesses' ethnic in-group/ out-group status. Methods. Participants (N = 120) rated the reliability of videotaped testimonies of high- and low-accurate in-group (Swedish, N = 4) and out-group (immigrants, N = 4) witnesses who were genuinely trying to recall a criminal event. Results. Participants assigned more reliability to high- than to low-accurate in-group witnesses, while out-group witnesses received low reliability ratings regardless of their actual memory performance. Path analyses demonstrated that the subjective confidence of in-group, but not of out-group, witnesses predicted participants' accuracy judgments. Conclusions. The results indicate that the validity in judgments of in-group witnesses can be better than has previously been implied. Investigators may have difficulty distinguishing high- and low-accurate witnesses from other ethnic groups, and they may also systematically underestimate the reliability of ethnic out-group witnesses. Implications for legal practices are discussed.
  •  
9.
  • Mac Giolla, Erik, 1987, et al. (författare)
  • What to do with all these Bayes factors: How to make Bayesian reports in deception research more informative
  • 2020
  • Ingår i: Legal and Criminological Psychology. - : Wiley. - 1355-3259 .- 2044-8333. ; :2, s. 65-71
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Bayes factors quantify the evidence in support of the null (absence of an effect) or the alternative hypothesis (presence of an effect). Based on commonly used cut‐offs, Bayes factors between 1/3 and 3 are interpreted as evidentially weak, and one typically concludes there is an absence of evidence. In this commentary on Warmelink, Subramanian, Tkacheva, and McLatchie (Legal Criminol Psychol 24, 2019, 258), we discuss how a Bayesian report can be made more informative. Firstly, this implies a departure from the labels provided by commonly used cut‐offs when reporting Bayes factors. Instead, we encourage researchers to report the value of the Bayes factors, or to convert these values into nominal support for the hypotheses. Secondly, researchers can provide recommendations to design follow‐up studies by examining the posterior distribution of the magnitude of the effect size. Lastly, we show how individual Bayes factors can be evaluated in the context of large‐scale meta‐analyses.
  •  
10.
  • Magnusson, Mikaela, 1992, et al. (författare)
  • Preschoolers' true and false reports: Comparing effects of the Sequential Interview and NICHD protocol
  • 2021
  • Ingår i: Legal and Criminological Psychology. - : Wiley. - 1355-3259 .- 2044-8333. ; 26:1, s. 83-102
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Purpose. The current study aimed to examine a Norwegian technique for conducting investigative interviews with preschoolers: the Sequential Interview (SI). The SI advocates for increased initial rapport building and includes a predetermined break before the substantive phase. To explore the potential benefits and risks of the SI, the technique was compared with an adapted version of the National Institute of Child Health and Development (NICHD) protocol. Methods. A total of 129 preschoolers (3-6 years) were interviewed with either the SI or NICHD protocol about a self-experienced (Exp. I) or non-experienced (Exp. II) event. Result. For Exp. I, no significant difference was observed across interview conditions in the number of reported details about a self-experienced event. Children interviewed with the SI exhibited a slightly lower accuracy rate compared to those interviewed with the NICHD protocol. For Exp. II, a total of 31.1% of the preschoolers initially assented to remembering a fictive (false) experience and 15.6% gave an account (>40 details) of the non-experienced event. We found no difference between interviewing conditions in assent rates or number of false accounts. Conclusions. The study provides valuable insights into the difficulties involved when interviewing young children. The results showed few differences between the novel SI model and the well-established NICHD protocol. While many preschoolers could provide accurate testimony, some embedded worrisome false details in their narratives. Furthermore, a minority of children gave false reports about non-experienced events when interviewed with the two techniques. Methodological limitations and suggestions for future research will be discussed.
  •  
Skapa referenser, mejla, bekava och länka
  • Resultat 1-10 av 29
Typ av publikation
tidskriftsartikel (29)
Typ av innehåll
refereegranskat (28)
övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt (1)
Författare/redaktör
Granhag, Pär-Anders, ... (14)
Strömwall, Leif, 196 ... (8)
Landström, Sara, 198 ... (5)
Ernberg, Emelie, 198 ... (4)
Willén, Rebecca M., ... (4)
Mac Giolla, Erik, 19 ... (3)
visa fler...
Vrij, Aldert (3)
Ask, Karl, 1978 (2)
Strand, Susanne (2)
Belfrage, Henrik (2)
Vrij, A. (2)
Magnusson, Mikaela, ... (2)
Fisher, R. P. (1)
Shaw, D (1)
Leal, S (1)
Luke, Timothy, 1989 (1)
Alfredsson, Helen, 1 ... (1)
Granhag, Pär Anders (1)
Lindholm, Torun (1)
Andersson, L-O (1)
Archer, John (1)
Dixon, Louise (1)
Graham-Kevan, Nicola (1)
Tidefors, Inga, 1949 (1)
Öhman, Lisa (1)
Hartwig, Maria, 1981 (1)
Korkman, Julia (1)
Verschuere, B (1)
Fransson, Göran (1)
Pettersson, Daniel (1)
Nyström, Lina, 1994 (1)
Roos Af Hjelmsäter, ... (1)
Hillman, J. (1)
Cancino Montecinos, ... (1)
Mann, S (1)
Masip, J (1)
Hartwig, M. (1)
Hartwig, Maria (1)
Levander, S (1)
Horselenberg, R (1)
Hope, L (1)
Hope, Lorraine (1)
Deeb, Haneen, 1984 (1)
Mann, Samantha (1)
Leal, Sharon (1)
Kleinman, Steven M. (1)
Fransson, G (1)
Magnusson, Mikaela (1)
Ly, Alexander (1)
Sooniste, Tuule, 198 ... (1)
visa färre...
Lärosäte
Göteborgs universitet (24)
Mittuniversitetet (3)
Linnéuniversitetet (3)
Örebro universitet (2)
Luleå tekniska universitet (1)
Stockholms universitet (1)
visa fler...
Högskolan Väst (1)
Mälardalens universitet (1)
Linköpings universitet (1)
Lunds universitet (1)
visa färre...
Språk
Engelska (29)
Forskningsämne (UKÄ/SCB)
Samhällsvetenskap (28)
Teknik (1)
Medicin och hälsovetenskap (1)

År

Kungliga biblioteket hanterar dina personuppgifter i enlighet med EU:s dataskyddsförordning (2018), GDPR. Läs mer om hur det funkar här.
Så här hanterar KB dina uppgifter vid användning av denna tjänst.

 
pil uppåt Stäng

Kopiera och spara länken för att återkomma till aktuell vy