SwePub
Tyck till om SwePub Sök här!
Sök i SwePub databas

  Utökad sökning

Träfflista för sökning "WFRF:(Rönnlund Michael) "

Sökning: WFRF:(Rönnlund Michael)

  • Resultat 1-10 av 75
Sortera/gruppera träfflistan
   
NumreringReferensOmslagsbildHitta
1.
  • Davis, Paul A., et al. (författare)
  • Balancing time for health behaviors : associations of time perspective with physical activity and weight management in older adults
  • 2024
  • Ingår i: American Journal of Health Promotion. - : Sage Publications. - 0890-1171 .- 2168-6602.
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Purpose: To examine associations between time perspective and health promotion behaviors of physical activity and weight management.Design: Quantitative cross-sectional.Setting: This study is part of the Betula project on aging, memory, and dementia in Northern Sweden.Subjects: 417 older adults aged between 55 and 85 years.Measures: Swedish-Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory; Physical Activity in the past year, past week, and in comparison with others of similar age; Weight Management = Body Mass Index (BMI; kg/m2).Results: After controlling for age, sex, and years of education, hierarchical linear regression indicated a Balanced Time Perspective was significantly associated with more physical activity in the past year (P =.04), the past week (P <.001), and in comparison with others (P <.01). Past Negative time perspective was associated with less physical activity in the past year (P =.03), and in comparison with others (P =.03). Present Fatalistic was associated with less physical activity during the past week (P =.03), and in comparison with others (P =.01). Present Hedonistic was associated with more physical activity the past week (P =.03), and in comparison with others (P =.03). Past Negative was associated with higher BMI (P =.02), and Future Negative were associated with lower BMI (P =.01). Taken collectively, greater positivity and flexibility across time perspectives was associated with more physical activity, whereas negative oriented time perspectives related with less physical activity and poorer weight management.Conclusion: Time perspective can be associated with health behaviors in older adults and have implications for health across the lifespan. Health promotion interventions may target older adults’ enjoyment of exercise and weight management in the present, rather than highlight potential negative health outcomes in the future. 
  •  
2.
  • Blomgren, Anna-Sara, et al. (författare)
  • Coping strategies in late adolescence : relationships to parental attachment and time perspective
  • 2016
  • Ingår i: The Journal of Genetic Psychology. - : Taylor & Francis. - 0022-1325 .- 1940-0896. ; 177:3, s. 85-96
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • The authors investigated adolescents' use of coping strategies in relation to attachment to parents and time perspective. Adolescents in Grade 3 upper secondary school (M age = 18.3 years, SD = 0.6 years; n = 160) completed the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment, the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory, and the Brief COPE. Correlational analyses showed that attachment to parents was associated with a more favorable view of the past (higher past positive and lower past negative), a less fatalistic view of the present, and a more favorable view of the future (higher future positive and lower future negative). Parental attachment accounted for significant variance in composite coping scores (adaptive and maladaptive) when entered before, but not after, time perspective subscales in hierarchical regression analyses. However, time perspective (mainly present hedonistic and positive or negative future) predicted adaptive or maladaptive coping over and beyond attachment. The results are consistent with the hypothesis that most of the relationship between adolescents' attachment to parents and coping is mediated by individual differences in time perspective. By contrast, factors other than attachment to parents (e.g., temperament) must be considered to fully account for the relationship between time perspective and coping.
  •  
3.
  • Börjesson, A, et al. (författare)
  • Linopirdine (DUP 996) : cholinergic treatment of older adults using successive and non-successive tests.
  • 1999
  • Ingår i: Neuropsychobiology. - 0302-282X .- 1423-0224. ; 40:2, s. 78-85
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • The purpose of this study was to examine whether cholinergic treatment of age-associated memory impairment with Linopirdine (DUP 996), a derivate of phenylindoline, affects explicit memory, implicit memory, and primary memory. We also assessed cognitive decision making in a reaction time test. Explicit memory was assessed by face recognition, word recall and a word recognition test, being part of a successive test paradigm. Implicit memory was assessed by primed word fragment completion in the same successive test paradigm. Primary memory was studied by means of digit recall. Thirty-eight elderly subjects fulfilled the criteria for memory impairment. Four groups of subjects were given 10, 20 or 30 mg of DUP 996 or placebo during 4 weeks. A double-blind procedure was applied. No significant treatment effects for recognition memory and priming were obtained in the successive test paradigm. Analysis of dependence/independence between tests did not show any clear pattern of treatment effects. The other explicit memory tests and the reaction time test showed no effect with DUP 996. Because of the range of the different tests used here, the result and the general evidence in other investigations of the cholinergic depletion among aged people, the conclusion is that DUP 996 does not improve memory performance either in explicit, implicit or primary tests.
  •  
4.
  • Ekström, Ingrid, 1988-, et al. (författare)
  • Subjective Olfactory Loss in Older Adults Concurs with Long-Term Odor Identification Decline
  • 2019
  • Ingår i: Chemical Senses. - Oxford : Oxford University Press (OUP). - 0379-864X .- 1464-3553. ; 44:2, s. 105-112
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Olfactory impairments may provide early indications of future health outcomes in older adults. Thus, an important question concerns whether these impairments can be self-assessed. Previous findings of cross-sectional studies indicate low correlations between self-reported olfactory function and objective olfactory performance. On the other hand, subjective olfactory impairments predict future dementia and mortality in longitudinal settings. No previous study has assessed the relationship between subjectively and objectively measured decline in olfaction over time. Based on data for 903 older adults derived from the Betula Study, a Swedish population-based prospective study, we tested whether rate-of-change in odor identification could be predicted from subjective olfactory decline over a time span of 10 years during which subjective and objective odor functions were assessed on 2 or 3 test occasions. Indeed, we found that participants who experienced subjective olfactory decline over the study period also had significantly steeper rates of decline in odor identification, even after adjusting for demographic, cognitive, and genetic factors that previously have been associated with performance in odor identification. This association was, however, not present in a subsample with baseline cognitive impairment. We interpret these results as evidence that when asked about whether they have an olfactory impairment or not, older persons are assessing intraindividual olfactory changes, rather than interindividual differences. Our results indicate that subjective olfactory loss reflects objective olfactory decline in cognitively intact older adults. This association might be harnessed to predict health outcomes and highlights the need to develop effective olfactory self-assessments.
  •  
5.
  • Eriksson, Daniel, et al. (författare)
  • Effects of perceived long-term stress on health and memory functioning
  • 2010
  • Ingår i: Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society. ; , s. 78-78
  • Konferensbidrag (refereegranskat)abstract
    • The study examined effects of perceived long-term stress on health and memory functioning in middle-aged individuals (40–60 years). Participants in the Betula study (Nilsson et al., 1997) describing themselves as being stressed in general over three measurement occasions (10 years in total) were compared with a matched (sex and education) group (n = 98) reporting no stress. The results revealed a higher incidence of depressive symptoms, flus, and not-healthy-ratings over time for the stress group. In addition, the stress group provided more negative subjective memory ratings, whereas time-related change in memory performance, indicative of a high degree of cognitive stability, did not differ from that of controls. Degree of perceived stress is discussed as a factor underlying variations in regard to the outcome of studies of perceived stress.
  •  
6.
  • Eriksson Sörman, Daniel, et al. (författare)
  • Blood Pressure Levels and Longitudinal Changes in Relation to Social Network Factors
  • 2016
  • Ingår i: Psychological Topics. - 1332-0742. ; 25:1, s. 59-73
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • The aim of this study was to examine the relationship between social network variables and levels of and longitudinal changes in blood pressure in a middle-aged/older sample. The participants (50-75 years at baseline; n=1097) responded to questions concerning social relationships at baseline and their blood pressure (diastolic, systolic) was measured. Blood pressure levels were reassessed 5, 10, and 15 years later. Latent growth models with responses to questions concerning social relationships as predictors and basic demographic factors (age, sex) as covariates, unexpectedly indicated that a more limited social network (no close friend, few visits, little contact with friends in other ways, not living with someone, and a composite index based on all questions) was associated with significantly lower diastolic blood pressure levels. For systolic blood pressure a similar result was observed for one of the variables (lack of a close friend). In general, these effects diminished over time, as indexed by the positive relationship between several of the social variables and slope. The results were little affected by inclusion of additional covariates (e.g. measures of psychological distress, smoking/alcohol habits, and BMI) suggesting that the origins of this unexpected pattern of findings must probably be sought for in other subjectrelated factors, such as, for example, increased help seeking. Future studies should consider qualitative aspects (e.g. feelings of loneliness, quality of social relationships) in addition to structural aspects to provide a better understanding of these associations.
  •  
7.
  • Eriksson Sörman, Daniel, et al. (författare)
  • Ger en aktiv livsstil bättre minne?
  • 2011
  • Ingår i: Svensk Idrottsforskning. - Stockholm : Centrum för idrottsforskning. - 1103-4629. ; 20:1, s. 43-45
  • Tidskriftsartikel (övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt)abstract
    • Det sägs ibland att aktivteter som stimulerar hjärnan förbättrar minnet. En rad studier indikerar också att olika typer av livsstilsfaktorer hänger samman med prestation i kognitiva test. De visar att dålig minnesförmåga är överrepresenterad hos dem som inte ägnar sig åt fritidsaktiviteter såsom att läsa, idrotta och lägga pussel.
  •  
8.
  • Eriksson Sörman, Daniel, et al. (författare)
  • Leisure Activity in Old Age and Risk of Dementia : a 15-Year Prospective Study
  • 2014
  • Ingår i: The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences. - : Oxford University Press. - 1079-5014 .- 1758-5368. ; 69:4, s. 493-501
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Objectives. The aim of this study was to investigate whether leisure activity is associated with incident dementia in an older sample.Method. We examined a sample of 1,475 elderly (>= 65 years) who were dementia free at baseline over a follow-up period of up to 15 years. In addition to analyses involving the total time period, separate analyses of three time periods were performed, 1-5, 6-10, and 11-15 years, following baseline measurement of leisure activity.Results. After controlling for a variety of potential confounders, analyses of data for the total time period revealed that higher levels of "Total activity" and "Social activity," but not "Mental activity," were associated with decreased risk of dementia. However, analyses of the separate time periods showed that this association was only significant in the first time period, 1-5 years after baseline.Discussion. The results from this study provide little support for the hypothesis that frequent engagement in leisure activities among elderly serve to protect against dementia diseases across a longer time frame. The finding of a relationship for the first time period, 1-5 years after baseline, could indicate short-term protective effects but could also reflect reverse causality.
  •  
9.
  • Eriksson Sörman, Daniel, et al. (författare)
  • Leisure-time activity in old age as predictors of impending dementia: A 15-year prospective study
  • 2012
  • Konferensbidrag (refereegranskat)abstract
    • This study examined the relationship between leisure activities and risk of dementia in a sample of healthy older individuals, dementia free at the beginning of the project. Data were drawn from a population-based longitudinal study (the Betula project) and the participants were followed up for 15 years. At baseline, participants were asked about their frequency of participation in 15 selected leisure activities. When age, gender, education, APOE and other potential confounders were controlled for, results revealed quite moderate effects on dementia after analysis of the activities separately. However, by weighting each activity into a mental, social and physical dimension (based on valuation by the participants), and then summarizing into a score for each dimension, we further investigated if level of engagement could predict impending dementia. Preliminary results indicate that the dimensions may have influence on the risk of dementia for certain age groups. The study also showed that the strongest predictor of dementia is being a carrier of the APOE ɛ4 allele. The outcomes are discussed in terms of important methodological difference between studies concerning the effects of leisure activities in preventing dementia diseases.
  •  
10.
  • Eriksson Sörman, Daniel, 1974-, et al. (författare)
  • Occupational cognitive complexity and episodic memory in old age
  • 2021
  • Ingår i: Intelligence. - : Elsevier. - 0160-2896 .- 1873-7935. ; 89
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • The aim of this study was to investigate occupational cognitive complexity of main lifetime occupation in relation to level and 15-year change in episodic memory recall in a sample of older adults (≥ 65 years, n = 780). We used latent growth curve modelling with occupational cognitive complexity (O*NET indicators) as independent variable. Subgroup analyses in a sample of middle-aged (mean: 49.9 years) men (n = 260) were additionally performed to investigate if a general cognitive ability (g) factor at age 18 was predictive of future occupational cognitive complexity and cognitive performance in midlife. For the older sample, a higher level of occupational cognitive complexity was related to a higher level of episodic recall (β = 0.15, p < .001), but the association with rate of change (β = 0.03, p = .64) was not statistically significant. In the middle-aged sample, g at age 18 was both directly (β = 0.19, p = .01) and indirectly (via years of education after age 18, ab = 0.19) predictive of midlife levels of occupational cognitive complexity. Cognitive ability at age 18 was also a direct predictor of midlife episodic recall (β = 0.60, p ≤ 0.001). Critically, entry of the early adult g factor attenuated the association between occupational complexity and cognitive level (from β = 0.21, p = .01 to β = 0.12, p = .14). Overall, our results support a pattern of preserved differentiation from early to late adulthood for individuals with different histories of occupational complexity.
  •  
Skapa referenser, mejla, bekava och länka
  • Resultat 1-10 av 75
Typ av publikation
tidskriftsartikel (66)
konferensbidrag (4)
doktorsavhandling (4)
forskningsöversikt (1)
Typ av innehåll
refereegranskat (67)
övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt (8)
Författare/redaktör
Rönnlund, Michael (36)
Nilsson, Lars-Göran (32)
Sundström, Anna (12)
Adolfsson, Rolf (11)
Åström, Elisabeth (9)
Nyberg, Lars, 1966- (5)
visa fler...
Lövdén, Martin (5)
Nyberg, Lars (4)
Carelli, Maria Grazi ... (3)
Kormi-Nouri, Reza, 1 ... (2)
Larsson, Maria (2)
Karlsson, E (2)
Nordin, Steven, 1960 ... (2)
Olofsson, Jonas K. (2)
Josefsson, Maria, 19 ... (2)
Kauppi, Karolina (2)
Eriksson, Daniel (1)
Larsson, L (1)
Kliegel, Matthias (1)
Nilsson, L. (1)
Lövheim, Hugo, 1981- (1)
Pudas, Sara (1)
Backman, Lars (1)
Karlsson, T (1)
Nordin, M. (1)
Wahlin, Åke (1)
Norberg, Margareta (1)
Bäckman, Lars (1)
Davis, Paul A. (1)
Oudin, Anna (1)
Weinehall, Lars (1)
Lyxell, Björn, Profe ... (1)
Pudas, Sara, Docent, ... (1)
Lindström, T. (1)
Stenling, Andreas, 1 ... (1)
Sternäng, Ola (1)
Nilsson, L-G (1)
Herlitz, Agneta (1)
Lundquist, Anders, 1 ... (1)
Johansson, Boo, prof ... (1)
Vega Mendoza, Marian ... (1)
Boraxbekk, Carl-Joha ... (1)
Juslin, Peter (1)
Bergdahl, Jan (1)
Bergdahl, Maud (1)
Blomgren, Anna-Sara (1)
Svahn, Kajsa (1)
Blomstedt, Yulia (1)
Börjesson, A. (1)
Carlstedt, Berit (1)
visa färre...
Lärosäte
Umeå universitet (69)
Stockholms universitet (27)
Luleå tekniska universitet (11)
Karolinska Institutet (10)
Örebro universitet (2)
Uppsala universitet (1)
visa fler...
Lunds universitet (1)
Karlstads universitet (1)
Försvarshögskolan (1)
visa färre...
Språk
Engelska (73)
Svenska (2)
Forskningsämne (UKÄ/SCB)
Samhällsvetenskap (60)
Medicin och hälsovetenskap (14)
Teknik (3)

År

Kungliga biblioteket hanterar dina personuppgifter i enlighet med EU:s dataskyddsförordning (2018), GDPR. Läs mer om hur det funkar här.
Så här hanterar KB dina uppgifter vid användning av denna tjänst.

 
pil uppåt Stäng

Kopiera och spara länken för att återkomma till aktuell vy