SwePub
Sök i SwePub databas

  Extended search

Träfflista för sökning "WFRF:(Västfjäll Daniel 1975 ) "

Search: WFRF:(Västfjäll Daniel 1975 )

  • Result 1-10 of 109
Sort/group result
   
EnumerationReferenceCoverFind
1.
  • Larsson, Pontus, 1974, et al. (author)
  • When what you see is what you hear: Auditory-visual integration and presence in virtual environments
  • 2007
  • In: Proceedings of the 10th Annual International Workshop on Presence, October 25-27, 2007, Barcelona, Spain. ; , s. 11-18
  • Conference paper (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • In this paper, it is hypothesized that consistency across modalities in terms of matching the visual space to the auditory space is important for the sense of presence. An experiment was carried out where thirty participants were exposed to four conditions having different degrees of auditory-visual consistency (one purely visual and three auditory-visual). A presence questionnaire was used after exposure to measure participants’ sensations. Although participants rated the auditory-visual conditions as inducing significantly higher presence than the condition with only visual information, no differences in presence ratings between the three auditory-visual conditions were found. However, participants’ rankings of their sensed presence in all conditions revealed that there might be such differences. Moreover, the results show that sound in general has a significant effect on VE users’ sense of presence.
  •  
2.
  • Van Bavel, Jay J., et al. (author)
  • National identity predicts public health support during a global pandemic
  • 2022
  • In: Nature Communications. - : Nature Portfolio. - 2041-1723. ; 13:1
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Understanding collective behaviour is an important aspect of managing the pandemic response. Here the authors show in a large global study that participants that reported identifying more strongly with their nation reported greater engagement in public health behaviours and support for public health policies in the context of the pandemic. Changing collective behaviour and supporting non-pharmaceutical interventions is an important component in mitigating virus transmission during a pandemic. In a large international collaboration (Study 1, N = 49,968 across 67 countries), we investigated self-reported factors associated with public health behaviours (e.g., spatial distancing and stricter hygiene) and endorsed public policy interventions (e.g., closing bars and restaurants) during the early stage of the COVID-19 pandemic (April-May 2020). Respondents who reported identifying more strongly with their nation consistently reported greater engagement in public health behaviours and support for public health policies. Results were similar for representative and non-representative national samples. Study 2 (N = 42 countries) conceptually replicated the central finding using aggregate indices of national identity (obtained using the World Values Survey) and a measure of actual behaviour change during the pandemic (obtained from Google mobility reports). Higher levels of national identification prior to the pandemic predicted lower mobility during the early stage of the pandemic (r = -0.40). We discuss the potential implications of links between national identity, leadership, and public health for managing COVID-19 and future pandemics.
  •  
3.
  • Vlasceanu, Madalina, et al. (author)
  • Addressing climate change with behavioral science: A global intervention tournament in 63 countries
  • 2024
  • In: Science Advances. - : American Association for the Advancement of Science (AAAS). - 2375-2548. ; 10:6
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Effectively reducing climate change requires marked, global behavior change. However, it is unclear which strategies are most likely to motivate people to change their climate beliefs and behaviors. Here, we tested 11 expert-crowdsourced interventions on four climate mitigation outcomes: beliefs, policy support, information sharing intention, and an effortful tree-planting behavioral task. Across 59,440 participants from 63 countries, the interventions’ effectiveness was small, largely limited to nonclimate skeptics, and differed across outcomes: Beliefs were strengthened mostly by decreasing psychological distance (by 2.3%), policy support by writing a letter to a future-generation member (2.6%), information sharing by negative emotion induction (12.1%), and no intervention increased the more effortful behavior—several interventions even reduced tree planting. Last, the effects of each intervention differed depending on people’s initial climate beliefs. These findings suggest that the impact of behavioral climate interventions varies across audiences and target behaviors.
  •  
4.
  • Andersson, Per A, 1986- (author)
  • Norms in Prosocial Decisions : The Role of Observability, Avoidance, and Conditionality
  • 2022
  • Doctoral thesis (other academic/artistic)abstract
    • Prosocial behaviors benefit other people and range from donations to charity to behavior limiting the spread of disease, such as masking and vaccination. The overarching purpose of this thesis was to contribute to our understanding of how social norms and conformity affect prosocial behavior. Here, three norm-related factors that affect such prosocial behavior were investigated: observability, avoidance and conditionality. Observability concerns whether a person is being observed during prosocial decisions, which can typically increase conformity to norms. Avoidance concerns whether a person avoids or seeks out knowledge about prosocial norms. Conditionality concerns the conditional nature of when behavior shifts occur in relation to others behavior. For instance, a person may want to follow a prosocial norm only if a very large majority adheres to it, or only if the goal of the norm is realistic to attain. Paper I focused on observability of prosocial decisions. Making decisions while knowing they would be shown to others increased prosocial behavior in the form of cooperation in a Public Goods Game, and preferences for deontological choices in moral dilemmas, but not donations given to charity. Paper II examined the existence of avoidance behavior regarding social norm about donations. Such norm avoiders appeared to be comprised of both prosocial and less prosocial individuals. Paper III investigated the interplay between descriptive (what people do) and injunctive (what one should do) norms in regards to masking during COVID-19. Paper IV then explored how varying the goal set for a prosocial norm affects willingness to try to achieve the goal, in the context of thresholds for herd immunity and vaccines for COVID-19. Some individuals were demotivated by seeing a higher goal as harder to achieve and others were motivated by believing a higher goal to lead to more people getting vaccinated. Taken together, these papers point to the inherent complexity of how norms relate to prosocial behavior, and suggest relevant aspects to consider when wanting to promote prosocial behavior. 
  •  
5.
  • Andersson, Per A, 1986-, et al. (author)
  • Prosocial and moral behavior under decision reveal in a public environment
  • 2020
  • In: Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics. - : Elsevier. - 2214-8043 .- 2214-8051. ; 87
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • People may act differently in public environments due to actual reputation concerns, or due to the mere presence of others. Unlike previous studies on the influence of observability on prosocial behavior we control for the latter while manipulating the former, i.e. we control for implicit reputation concerns while manipulating explicit. We show that revealing decisions in public did not affect altruistic behavior, while it increased cooperation and made subjects less likely to make utilitarian judgments in sacrificial dilemmas (i.e., harming one to save many). Our findings are in line with theoretical models suggesting that people, at large, are averse to standing out in both positive and negative ways when it comes to altruistic giving. This "wallflower effect" does however not seem to extend to decisions on cooperation and moral judgments made in public.
  •  
6.
  • Asutay, Erkin, 1982, et al. (author)
  • Attentional and emotional prioritization of the sounds occurring outside the visual field
  • 2015
  • In: Emotion. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 1528-3542 .- 1931-1516. ; 15:3, s. 281-286
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • The ability to detect and localize sounds in an environment is critical for survival. Localizing sound sources is a computational challenge for the human brain because the auditory cortex seems to lack a topographical space representation. However, attention and task demands can modulate localization performance. Here, we investigated whether the localization performance for sounds occurring directly in front of or behind people could be modulated by emotional salience and sound-source location. We measured auditory-induced emotion by ecological sounds occurring in the frontal or rear perceptual fields, and employed a speeded localization task. The results showed that both localization speed and accuracy were higher, and that stronger negative emotions were induced when sound sources were behind the participants. Our results provide clear behavioral evidence that auditory attention can be influenced by sound-source location. Importantly, we also show that the effect of spatial location on attention is mediated by emotion, which is in line with the argument that emotional information is prioritized in processing. Auditory system functions as an alarm system and is in charge of detecting possible salient events, and alarming for an attention shift. Further, spatial processing in the auditory dorsal pathway has a function of guiding the visual system to a particular location of interest. Thus, an auditory bias toward the space outside the visual field can be useful, so that visual attention could be quickly shifted in case of emotionally significant information.
  •  
7.
  • Asutay, Erkin, 1982, et al. (author)
  • Auditory attentional selection is biased by reward cues
  • 2016
  • In: Scientific Reports. - : Springer Science and Business Media LLC. - 2045-2322 .- 2045-2322. ; 6
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Auditory attention theories suggest that humans are able to decompose the complex acoustic input into separate auditory streams, which then compete for attentional resources. How this attentional competition is influenced by motivational salience of sounds is, however, not well-understood. Here, we investigated whether a positive motivational value associated with sounds could bias the attentional selection in an auditory detection task. Participants went through a reward-learning period, where correct attentional selection of one stimulus (CS+) lead to higher rewards compared to another stimulus (CS-). We assessed the impact of reward-learning by comparing perceptual sensitivity before and after the learning period, when CS+ and CS-were presented as distractors for a different target. Performance decreased after reward-learning when CS+ was a distractor, while it increased when CS- was a distractor. Thus, the findings show that sounds that were associated with high rewards captures attention involuntarily. Additionally, when successful inhibition of a particular sound (CS-) was associated with high rewards then it became easier to ignore it. The current findings have important implications for the understanding of the organizing principles of auditory perception and provide, for the first time, clear behavioral evidence for reward-dependent attentional learning in the auditory domain in humans.
  •  
8.
  • Asutay, Erkin, 1982, et al. (author)
  • Emoacoustics : A Study of the Psychoacoustical and Psychological Dimensions of Emotional Sound Design
  • 2012
  • In: Journal of The Audio Engineering Society. - : Audio Engineering Society. - 1549-4950. ; 60:1-2, s. 21-28
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Even though traditional psychoacoustics has provided indispensable knowledge about auditory perception, it has, in its narrow focus on signal characteristics, neglected listener and contextual characteristics. To demonstrate the influence of the meaning the listener attaches to a sound in the resulting sensations we used a Fourier-time-transform processing to reduce the identifiability of 18 environmental sounds. In a listening experiment, 20 subjects listened to and rated their sensations in response to, first, all the processed stimuli and then, all original stimuli, without being aware of the relationship between the two groups. Another 20 subjects rated only the processed stimuli, which were primed by their original counterparts. This manipulation was used in order to see the difference in resulting sensation when the subject could tell what the sound source is. In both tests subjects rated their emotional experience for each stimulus on the orthogonal dimensions of valence and arousal, as well as perceived annoyance and perceived loudness for each stimulus. They were also asked to identify the sound source. It was found that processing caused correct identification to reduce substantially, while priming recovered most of the identification. While original stimuli induced a wide range of emotional experience, reactions to processed stimuli were emotionally neutral. Priming manipulation reversed the effects of processing to some extent. Moreover, even though the 5th percentile Zwickers-loudness (N5) value of most of the stimuli was reduced after processing, neither perceived loudness nor auditory-induced emotion changed accordingly. Thus indicating the importance of considering other factors apart from the physical sound characteristics in sound design.
  •  
9.
  • Asutay, Erkin, 1982, et al. (author)
  • Emoacoustics: a study on the physical and psychological dimensions of sound design
  • 2010
  • In: 3rd Int. Workshop on Perceptual Quality of Systems, 2010, Bautzen, Germany. - 1680-8908. ; , s. 35-39
  • Conference paper (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Psychoacoustical research provided indispensible knowledge on how human audition works, which is necessary for successful sound design applications. It may, however, be argued that traditional psychoacoustics, in its narrow focus on signal characteristics, has neglected listener and contextual characteristics. Thus, to demonstrate the influence of meaning the listener attaches to a sound we used an FFT processing to reduce the identifiability of 18 environmental sounds, since source identification is central to meaning attribution. In a listening experiment, 20 subjects listened to and rated all the processed stimuli first and then original stimuli, without being aware of the existence of two sets. Another 20 subjects rated only the processed stimuli, which were primed by their original counterparts. This manipulation was used in order to see the difference when the subject could tell what the sound source is. In both tests subjects rated their emotional experience for each stimulus on the orthogonal dimensions of valence and arousal, as well as perceived annoyance and perceived loudness for each stimulus. They were also asked to identify the sound source. It was found that processing caused correct identification to reduce substantially, while priming recovered most of the identification. While original stimuli induced a wide range of emotional experience, reactions to processed stimuli were affectively neutral. Priming manipulation reversed the effects of processing to some extent. Moreover, even though Zwickers-loudness value of most of the stimuli was reduced after processing, perceived loudness was only decreased for affectively negative stimuli.
  •  
10.
  • Asutay, Erkin, 1982, et al. (author)
  • Haptic sensation in organ playing
  • 2010
  • In: Haptic Audio Interaction Design 2010, Copenhagen, Denmark.
  • Conference paper (other academic/artistic)abstract
    • This paper presents a new research project, which aims toreveal the elements of haptic sensation in playing pipeorgan with mechanical tracker actions. In order to reach this goal sets of experiments will be carried out. Layout of these experiments are (1) measuring manual key signatures and analyzing them together with the interviews with expert organists to study the extent they can feel and communicate key properties, (2) measuring dynamical properties of key action, recording sound and recording organist’s movements simultaneously during performance, and (3) running psychophysical experiments on expert organists using an experimental keyboard where key action can be simulated and altered. The outcome of this research project will contribute to organ building and documentation and new class of haptics-enabled digital musical instruments.
  •  
Skapa referenser, mejla, bekava och länka
  • Result 1-10 of 109
Type of publication
journal article (71)
conference paper (19)
doctoral thesis (9)
book chapter (5)
reports (3)
editorial collection (1)
show more...
research review (1)
show less...
Type of content
peer-reviewed (87)
other academic/artistic (22)
Author/Editor
Västfjäll, Daniel, 1 ... (103)
Tinghög, Gustav, 197 ... (27)
Kleiner, Mendel, 194 ... (21)
Tajadura, Ana, 1979 (14)
Larsson, Pontus, 197 ... (13)
Bergman, Penny, 1982 (11)
show more...
Asutay, Erkin, 1982 (10)
Genell, Anders, 1974 ... (9)
Barrafrem, Kinga, 19 ... (9)
Fransson, Niklas, 19 ... (8)
Sköld, Anders, 1976 (8)
Gärling, Tommy, 1941 (7)
Erlandsson, Arvid, 1 ... (7)
Slovic, Paul (7)
Västfjäll, Daniel, P ... (6)
Dickert, Stephan (5)
Bjälkebring, Pär, 19 ... (5)
Peters, Ellen (5)
Aczel, Balazs (3)
Andersson, David, 19 ... (3)
Tinghög, Gustav, Ass ... (3)
Nilsson, Artur (3)
Baskin, Ernest (3)
Holzmeister, Felix (2)
Huber, Juergen (2)
Kirchler, Michael (2)
Ahmed, Ali M., Profe ... (2)
Andersson, Per A, 19 ... (2)
Vanpaemel, Wolf (2)
Voracek, Martin (2)
Andersson, Gerhard, ... (2)
Erlandsson, Arvid, S ... (2)
Skagerlund, Kenny, 1 ... (2)
Azevedo, Flavio (2)
Alfano, Mark (2)
Vlasceanu, Madalina (2)
Bialek, Michal (2)
Borau, Sylvie (2)
Buchel, Ondrej (2)
Rathje, Steve (2)
Boggio, Paulo S. (2)
Slovic, P. (2)
Roets, Arne (2)
Mechtel, Mario (2)
Scopelliti, Irene (2)
Warmelink, Lara (2)
Szecsi, Peter (2)
Kovacs, Marton (2)
Wiggins, Bradford J. (2)
Bakos, Bence E. (2)
show less...
University
Chalmers University of Technology (67)
Linköping University (48)
University of Gothenburg (23)
Stockholm University (2)
Stockholm School of Economics (2)
Karolinska Institutet (2)
show more...
Umeå University (1)
Uppsala University (1)
Lund University (1)
VTI - The Swedish National Road and Transport Research Institute (1)
show less...
Language
English (108)
Swedish (1)
Research subject (UKÄ/SCB)
Social Sciences (81)
Engineering and Technology (28)
Natural sciences (9)
Medical and Health Sciences (8)
Humanities (2)

Year

Kungliga biblioteket hanterar dina personuppgifter i enlighet med EU:s dataskyddsförordning (2018), GDPR. Läs mer om hur det funkar här.
Så här hanterar KB dina uppgifter vid användning av denna tjänst.

 
pil uppåt Close

Copy and save the link in order to return to this view