SwePub
Sök i SwePub databas

  Extended search

Träfflista för sökning "L773:0096 1523 OR L773:1939 1277 "

Search: L773:0096 1523 OR L773:1939 1277

  • Result 1-31 of 31
Sort/group result
   
EnumerationReferenceCoverFind
1.
  •  
2.
  • Madison, Guy, 1961- (author)
  • Variability in isochronous tapping: higher-order dependencies as a function of inter tap interval
  • 2001
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 27:2, s. 411-422
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Isochronous serial interval production (ISIP) data, as from unpaced finger tapping, exhibit higher-order dependencies (drift). This fact has largely been ignored by the timing literature, one reason probably being that influential timing models assume random variability. Men and women, 22-36 years old, performed a synchronization-continuation task with inter-tap intervals (ITI) from 0.4 s to 2.2 s. ISIP variability was partitioned into components attributable to drift and first-order serial correlation, and the results indicate that (a) drift contributes substantially to the dispersion for longer ITIs, (b) drift and first-order correlation are different functions of the ITI, and (c) drift exhibits breaks close to 1 s and 1.4 s ITI. These breaks correspond to qualitative changes in performance for other temporal tasks, which suggests common timing processes across modalities and tasks.
  •  
3.
  • Runeson, Sverker, 1942-, et al. (author)
  • Achievement of Specificational Information Usage with True and False Feedback in Learning a Visual Relative-Mass Discrimination Task
  • 2007
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 33:1, s. 163-182
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Participants' usage of informational variables in learning visual relative-mass discrimination in collisions was tracked by means of PROBIT correlations. Four groups received feedback that was true or accorded with either of three non-specificational cue variables. A majority in each group adopted the feedback but several participants defied the false feedback. Unlike in previous research, the fit to data of the relative-mass invariant could not be bettered by post hoc linear combinations of the cues. Discriminability was lower in the use of the invariant. Analytic complexity was rejected as an explanation for discriminability differences. A "smart mechanism" for pickup of the relative-mass invariant was developed as an extension of G. Johansson's (1950/1994) vector model.
  •  
4.
  • Collier, Elizabeth S, et al. (author)
  • It's out of my hands! grasping capacity may not influence perceived object size
  • 2017
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association Inc.. - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 43:4, s. 749-769
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Linkenauger, Witt, and Proffitt (2011) found that the perceived size of graspable objects was scaled by perceived grasping capacity. However, it is possible that this effect occurred because object size was estimated on the same trial as grasping capacity. This may have led to a conflation of estimates of perceived action capacity and spatial properties. In 5 experiments, we tested Linkenauger et al.'s claim that right-handed observers overestimate the grasping capacity of their right hand relative to their left hand, and that this, in turn, leads them to underestimate the size of objects to-be-grasped in their right hand relative to their left hand. We replicated the finding that right handers overestimate the size and grasping capacity of their right hand relative to their left hand. However, when estimates of object size and grasping capacity were made in separate tasks, objects grasped in the right hand were not underestimated relative to those grasped in the left hand. Further, when grasping capacity was physically restricted, observers appropriately recalibrated their perception of their maximum grasp but estimates of object size were unaffected. Our results suggest that changes in action capacity may not influence perceived object size if sources of conflation are controlled for. 
  •  
5.
  •  
6.
  •  
7.
  • Hervais-Adelman, Alexis G., et al. (author)
  • Generalization of perceptual learning of vocoded speech
  • 2011
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 37:1, s. 283-295
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Recent work demonstrates that learning to understand noise-vocoded (NV) speech alters sublexical perceptual processes but is enhanced by the simultaneous provision of higher-level, phonological, but not lexical content (Hervais-Adelman, Davis, Johnsrude, & Carlyon, 2008), consistent with top-down learning (Davis, Johnsrude, Hervais-Adelman, Taylor, & McGettigan, 2005; Hervais-Adelman et al., 2008). Here, we investigate whether training listeners with specific types of NV speech improves intelligibility of vocoded speech with different acoustic characteristics. Transfer of perceptual learning would provide evidence for abstraction from variable properties of the speech input. In Experiment 1, we demonstrate that learning of NV speech in one frequency region generalizes to an untrained frequency region. In Experiment 2, we assessed generalization among three carrier signals used to create NV speech: noise bands, pulse trains, and sine waves. Stimuli created using these three carriers possess the same slow, time-varying amplitude information and are equated for naïve intelligibility but differ in their temporal fine structure. Perceptual learning generalized partially, but not completely, among different carrier signals. These results delimit the functional and neural locus of perceptual learning of vocoded speech. Generalization across frequency regions suggests that learning occurs at a stage of processing at which some abstraction from the physical signal has occurred, while incomplete transfer across carriers indicates that learning occurs at a stage of processing that is sensitive to acoustic features critical for speech perception (e.g., noise, periodicity).
  •  
8.
  • Holm, Linus, et al. (author)
  • Motor and Executive Control in Repetitive Timing of Brief Intervals
  • 2013
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 39:2, s. 365-380
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • We investigated the causal role of executive control functions in the production of brief time intervals by means of a concurrent task paradigm. To isolate the influence of executive functions on timing from motor coordination effects, we dissociated executive load from the number of effectors used in the dual task situation. In 3 experiments, participants produced isochronous intervals ranging from 524 to 2,000 ms with either the left or the right hand. The concurrent task consisted of the production of either a pseudorandom (high cognitive load) or a simple repeated (low cognitive load) spatial sequence of key presses, while also maintaining a regular temporal sequence. This task was performed with either a single hand (unimanual) or with both hands simultaneously (bimanual). Interference in terms of increased timing variability caused by the concurrent task was observed only in the bimanual condition. We verified that motor coordination in bimanual tasks alone could not account for the interference. Timing interference only appeared when (a) more than 1 effector was involved and (b) there were simultaneous task demands that recruited executive functions. Task interference was not seen if only 1 of these 2 conditions was met. Thus, our results suggest that executive functions are not directly involved in motor timing, but can indirectly affect timing performance when they are required to schedule complex motor coordination.
  •  
9.
  • Juslin, Patrik N, 1969- (author)
  • Cue utilization in communication of emotion in music performance : Relating performance to perception
  • 2000
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 26:6, s. 1797-1813
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • The goal of this study was to describe the utilization of acoustic cues in communication of emotions in music performance. Three professional guitarists were asked to perform 3 short melodies so as to communicate anger, sadness, happiness, and fear to listeners. The resulting performances were analyzed with respect to five acoustic cues and judged by 30 listeners on adjective scales. Multiple regression analysis was applied to the relationships between (a) the performer’s intention and the cues, and (b) the listeners’ judgments and the cues. The analyses of performers and listeners were related using Hursch, Hammond, and Hursch’s (1964) lens model equation. The results indicated that (a) performers were successful at communicating emotions to listeners, (b) performers’ cue utilization was well ”matched” to listeners’ cue utilization, and (c) cue utilization was more consistent across different melodies than across different performers. Due to the redundancy of the cues, two performers could communicate equally well despite differences in cue utilization.
  •  
10.
  • Lim, Sung-Joo, et al. (author)
  • Discovering Functional Units in Continuous Speech
  • 2015
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 41:4, s. 1139-1152
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Language learning requires that listeners discover acoustically variable functional units like phonetic categories and words from an unfamiliar, continuous acoustic stream. Although many category learning studies have examined how listeners learn to generalize across the acoustic variability inherent in the signals that convey the functional units of language, these studies have tended to focus upon category learning across isolated sound exemplars. However, continuous input presents many additional learning challenges that may impact category learning. Listeners may not know the timescale of the functional unit, its relative position in the continuous input, or its relationship to other evolving input regularities. Moving laboratory-based studies of isolated category exemplars toward more natural input is important to modeling language learning, but very little is known about how listeners discover categories embedded in continuous sound. In 3 experiments, adult participants heard acoustically variable sound category instances embedded in acoustically variable and unfamiliar sound streams within a video game task. This task was inherently rich in multisensory regularities with the to-be-learned categories and likely to engage procedural learning without requiring explicit categorization, segmentation, or even attention to the sounds. After 100 min of game play, participants categorized familiar sound streams in which target words were embedded and generalized this learning to novel streams as well as isolated instances of the target words. The findings demonstrate that even without a priori knowledge, listeners can discover input regularities that have the best predictive control over the environment for both non-native speech and nonspeech signals, emphasizing the generality of the learning.
  •  
11.
  • Lind, Mats, et al. (author)
  • Affine operations plus symmetry yield perception of metric shape with large perspective changes (≥ 45 degrees) : Data and model
  • 2014
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 40:1, s. 83-93
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • G. P. Bingham and M. Lind (2008, Large continuous perspective transformations are necessary and sufficient for accurate perception of metric shape, Perception & Psychophysics, Vol. 70, pp. 524-540) showed that observers could perceive metric shape, given perspective changes >= 45 degrees relative to a principal axis of elliptical cylinders. In this article, we tested (a) arbitrary perspective changes of 45 degrees, (b) whether perception gradually improves with more perspective change, (c) speed of rotation, (d) whether this works with other shapes (asymmetric polyhedrons), (e) different slants, and (f) perspective changes >45 degrees. Experiment 1 compared 45 degrees perspective change away from, versus centered on, a principal axis. Observers adjusted an ellipse to match the cross-section of an elliptical cylinder viewed in a stereo-motion display. Experiment 2 tested whether performance would improve gradually with increases in perspective change, or suddenly with a 45 degrees change. We also tested speed of rotation. Experiment 3 tested (a) asymmetric polyhedrons, (b) perspective change beyond 45 degrees, and (c) the effect of slant. The results showed (a) a particular perspective was not required, (b) judgments only improved with >= 45 degrees change, (c) speed was not relevant, (d) it worked with asymmetric polyhedrons, (e) slant was not relevant, and (f) judgments remained accurate beyond 45 degrees of change. A model shows how affine operations, together with a symmetry yielded by 45 degrees perspective change, bootstrap perception of metric shape.
  •  
12.
  • Madison, Guy, 1961-, et al. (author)
  • Modeling the tendency for music to induce movement in humans : First correlations with low-level audio descriptors across music genres
  • 2011
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association. - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 37:5, s. 1578-1594
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Groove is often described as the experience of music that makes people tap their feet and want to dance. A high degree of consistency in ratings of groove across listeners indicates that physical properties of the sound signal contribute to groove [Madison (2006) Music Perception, 24, 201-208]. Here, correlations were assessed between listener's ratings and a number of quantitative descriptors of rhythmic properties for one hundred music examples from five distinct traditional music genres. Groove was related to several different rhythmic properties, some of which were genre-specific and some of which were general across genres. Two descriptors corresponding to the density of events between beats and the salience of the beat, respectively, were strongly correlated with groove across domains. In contrast, systematic deviations from strict positions on the metrical grid, so-called microtiming, did not play any significant role. The results are discussed from a functional perspective of rhythmic music to enable and facilitate entrainment and precise synchronisation among individuals.
  •  
13.
  • Marsh, John E., et al. (author)
  • Dynamic cognitive control of irrelevant sound : increased task engagement attenuates semantic auditory distraction
  • 2015
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 41:5, s. 1462-1474
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Two experiments investigated reactive top-down cognitive control of the detrimental influence of spoken distractors semantically related to visually-presented words presented for free recall.  Experiment 1 demonstrated that an increase in focal task-engagement—promoted experimentally by reducing the perceptual discriminability of the visual target-words—eliminated the disruption by such distracters of veridical recall and also attenuated the erroneous recall of the distracters. A recall instruction that eliminates the requirement for output-monitoring was used in Experiment 2 to investigate whether increased task-engagement shields against distraction through a change in output-monitoring processes (back-end control) or by affecting the processing of the distracters during their presentation (front-end control). Rates of erroneous distracter-recall were much greater than in Experiment 1 but both erroneous distracter-recall and the disruptive effect of distracters on veridical recall were still attenuated under reduced target-word discriminability. Taken together, the results show that task-engagement is under dynamic strategic control and can be modulated to shield against auditory distraction by attenuating distracter-processing at encoding thereby preventing distracters from coming to mind at test.
  •  
14.
  • Olofsson, Jonas K., et al. (author)
  • High and Low Roads to Odor Valence? : A Choice Response-Time Study
  • 2013
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 39:5, s. 1205-1211
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Valence and edibility are two important features of olfactory perception, but it remains unclear how they are read out from an olfactory input. For a given odor object (e.g., the smell of rose or garlic), does perceptual identification of that object necessarily precede retrieval of information about its valence and edibility, or alternatively, are these processes independent? In the present study, we studied rapid, binary perceptual decisions regarding odor detection, object identity, valence, and edibility for a set of common odors. We found that decisions regarding odor-object identity were faster than decisions regarding odor valence or edibility, but slower than detection. Mediation analysis revealed that odor valence and edibility decision response times were predicted by a model in which odor-object identity served as a mediator along the perceptual pathway from detection to both valence and edibility. According to this model, odor valence is determined through both a low road that bypasses odor objects and a high road that utilizes odor-object information. Edibility evaluations are constrained to processing via the high road. The results outline a novel causal framework that explains how major perceptual features might be rapidly extracted from odors through engagement of odor objects early in the processing stream.
  •  
15.
  • Parmentier, Fabrice B. R., et al. (author)
  • A behavioral study of distraction by vibrotactile novelty
  • 2011
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 37:4, s. 1134-1139
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Past research has demonstrated that the occurrence of unexpected task-irrelevant changes in the auditory or visual sensory channels captured attention in an obligatory fashion, hindering behavioral performance in ongoing auditory or visual categorization tasks and generating orientation and re-orientation electrophysiological responses. We report the first experiment extending the behavioral study of cross-modal distraction to tactile novelty. Using a vibrotactile-visual cross-modal oddball task and a bespoke hand-arm vibration device, we found that participants were significantly slower at categorizing the parity of visually presented digits following a rare and unexpected change in vibrotactile stimulation (novelty distraction), and that this effect extended to the subsequent trial (postnovelty distraction). These results are in line with past research on auditory and visual novelty and fit the proposition of common and amodal cognitive mechanisms for the involuntary detection of change.
  •  
16.
  • Patching, Geoffrey, et al. (author)
  • Time- and space-order effects in timed discrimination of brightness and size of paired visual stimuli
  • 2012
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 38:4, s. 915-940
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Despite the importance of both response probability and response time for testing models of choice there is a dearth of chronometric studies examining systematic asymmetries that occur over time- and space-orders in the method of paired comparisons. In this study, systematic asymmetries in discriminating the magnitude of paired visual stimuli are examined by way of log-odds ratios of binary responses as well as by signed response speed. Hierarchical Bayesian modeling is used to map response probabilities and response speed onto constituent psychological process, and processing capacity is also assessed using response time distribution hazard functions. The findings include characteristic order effects that change systematically in magnitude and direction with changes in the magnitude and separation of the stimuli. After Hellström (1979, 2000), Sensation Weighting (SW) model analyses show that such order effects are reflected in the weighted accumulation of noisy information about the difference between stimulus values over time, and interindividual differences in weightings asymmetries are related to the relative processing capacity of participants. An account of sensation weighting based on the use of reference level information and maximization of signal-to-noise ratios is posited, which finds support from theoretically driven analyses of behavioral data.
  •  
17.
  • Poom, Leo (author)
  • Memory of Gender and Gait Direction From Biological Motion : Gender Fades Away But Directions Stay
  • 2012
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 38:5, s. 1091-1097
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • The delayed discrimination methodology has been used to demonstrate a high-fidelity nondecaying visual short-term memory (VSTM) for so-called preattentive basic features. In the current Study, I show that the nondecaying high VSTM precision is not restricted to basic features by using the same method to measure memory precision for gait direction and gender-stereotypical gait patterns from high-level point-light walkers. Nondecaying VSTM of direction was found for delays up to 9 s whereas memory for gender decayed. For both tasks, reaction times (RTs) increased with the delay, but only gender RT took longer when the two walkers faced different directions to the line of sight as compared to when they faced the same direction. The results may reflect differences between local and global processes, or an ecologically valid strategy where VSTM resources focus on variables that change, such as tracking people's movements, rather than variables that are constant during short timescales, such as gender.
  •  
18.
  • Poom, Leo, et al. (author)
  • Perceptual depth synthesis in the visual system as revealed by selective adaptation
  • 1999
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : AMER PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSOC. - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 25:2, s. 504-517
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Selective adaptation was used to determine the degree of interactions between channels processing relative depth from stereopsis. motion parallax, and texture. Monocular adaptations with motion parallax or binocular stationary adaptation caused test surf
  •  
19.
  •  
20.
  • Radziun, D, et al. (author)
  • Auditory cues influence the rubber-hand illusion
  • 2018
  • In: Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 1939-1277 .- 0096-1523. ; 44:7, s. 1012-1021
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)
  •  
21.
  •  
22.
  • Signoret, Carine, et al. (author)
  • Combined Effects of Form- and Meaning-Based Predictability on Perceived Clarity of Speech
  • 2018
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 44:2, s. 277-285
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • The perceptual clarity of speech is influenced by more than just the acoustic quality of the sound; it also depends on contextual support. For example, a degraded sentence is perceived to be clearer when the content of the speech signal is provided with matching text (i.e., form-based predictability) before hearing the degraded sentence. Here, we investigate whether sentence-level semantic coherence (i.e., meaning-based predictability), enhances perceptual clarity of degraded sentences, and if so, whether the mechanism is the same as that underlying enhancement by matching text. We also ask whether form- and meaning-based predictability are related to individual differences in cognitive abilities. Twenty participants listened to spoken sentences that were either clear or degraded by noise vocoding and rated the clarity of each item. The sentences had either high or low semantic coherence. Each spoken word was preceded by the homologous printed word (matching text), or by a meaningless letter string (nonmatching text). Cognitive abilities were measured with a working memory test. Results showed that perceptual clarity was significantly enhanced both by matching text and by semantic coherence. Importantly, high coherence enhanced the perceptual clarity of the degraded sentences even when they were preceded by matching text, suggesting that the effects of form- and meaning-based predictions on perceptual clarity are independent and additive. However, when working memory capacity indexed by the Size-Comparison Span Test was controlled for, only form-based predictions enhanced perceptual clarity, and then only at some sound quality levels, suggesting that prediction effects are to a certain extent dependent on cognitive abilities. (PsycINFO Database Record
  •  
23.
  • Terhune, Devin B., et al. (author)
  • Time Contracts and Temporal Precision Declines When the Mind Wanders
  • 2017
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 43:11, s. 1864-1871
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Our perception of time varies considerably from moment to moment, but how this variability relates to endogenous fluctuations in attentional states has been neglected. Here, we tested the hypothesis that perceptual decoupling during spontaneous mind wandering episodes distorts interval timing. In two studies with different visual subsecond interval timing paradigms, participants judged their attentional state on a trial-by-trial basis. Mind wandering states were characterized by underestimation of temporal intervals and a decline in temporal discrimination. Further analyses suggested that temporal contraction during mind wandering, but not the decline in temporal discrimination, could be attributed in part to attentional lapses. By contrast, we did not find any robust evidence that metacognition pertaining to interval timing was altered during mind wandering states. These results highlight the role of transient fluctuations in attentional states in intraindividual variability in time perception and have implications for the perceptual consequences, behavioral markers, and costs and benefits, of mind wandering.
  •  
24.
  • Van den Berg, Ronald, et al. (author)
  • Recent Is More : A Negative Time-Order Effect in Nonsymbolic Numerical Judgment.
  • 2017
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 43:6, s. 1084-1097
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Humans as well as some nonhuman animals can estimate object numerosities—such as the number of sheep in a flock—without explicit counting. Here, we report on a negative time-order effect (TOE) in this type of judgment: When nonsymbolic numerical stimuli are presented sequentially, the second stimulus is overestimated compared to the first. We examined this “recent is more” effect in two comparative judgment tasks: larger–smaller discrimination and same–different discrimination. Ideal-observer modeling revealed evidence for a TOE in 88.2% of the individual data sets. Despite large individual differences in effect size, there was strong consistency in effect direction: 87.3% of the identified TOEs were negative. The average effect size was largely independent of task but did strongly depend on both stimulus magnitude and interstimulus interval. Finally, we used an estimation task to obtain insight into the origin of the effect. We found that subjects tend to overestimate both stimuli but the second one more strongly than the first one. Overall, our findings are highly consistent with findings from studies on TOEs in nonnumerical judgments, which suggests a common underlying mechanism.
  •  
25.
  • von Hofsten, Claes, et al. (author)
  • Preparation for grasping an object: A developmental study.
  • 1988
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 14:4, s. 610-621
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • An optoelectronic technique (SELSPOT) was used to monitor the opening and closing of the hand during reaching actions by measuring the change in the distance between thumb and index finger. Exp 1 established an adult criterion for the development. Adults started closing the hand around the target well before touch, and the timing was dependent on the size of the target. The hand started to close earlier when grasping a small rather than a large target. In addition, the degree of hand opening was also less for a small than for a large target. In Exp 2, infants who were 5–6, 9, and 13 months of age also controlled their grasping actions visually and started closing the hand around the target in anticipation of the encounter rather than as a reaction to the encounter. The strategy of the two younger age groups was different from that of adults. They started closing the hand closer to the time of contact with the target than did the 13-month-olds, who were comparable to adults in this respect. In all age groups, reaching and grasping were most commonly organized in a continuous way; the hand started to close without any interruption in the approach. The opening of the hand was adjusted to target size in the 9- and 13-month-olds but not in the 5–6 month olds.
  •  
26.
  •  
27.
  • Wykowska, Agnieszka, et al. (author)
  • How You Move Is What You See : Action Planning Biases Selection in Visual Search
  • 2009
  • In: Journal of Experimental Psychology. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 0096-1523 .- 1939-1277. ; 35:6, s. 1755-1769
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Three experiments investigated the impact of planning and preparing a manual grasping or pointing movement on feature detection in a visual search task. The authors hypothesized that action planning may prime perceptual dimensions that provide information for the open parameters of that action. Indeed, preparing for grasping facilitated detection of size targets while preparing for pointing facilitated detection of luminance targets. Following the Theory of Event Coding (Hommel, Müsseler, Aschersleben, & Prinz, 2001b), the authors suggest that perceptual dimensions may be intentionally weighted with respect to an intended action. More interesting, the action-related influences were observed only when participants searched for a predefined target. This implies that action-related weighting is not independent from task-relevance weighting. To account for our findings, the authors suggest an integrative model of visual search that incorporates input from action-planning processes.
  •  
28.
  •  
29.
  •  
30.
  •  
31.
  • Madison, Guy (author)
  • Variability in isochronous tapping: Higher order dependencies as a function of intertap interval
  • 2001
  • In: JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-HUMAN PERCEPTION AND PERFORMANCE. - : AMER PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSOC. - 0096-1523. ; 27:2, s. 411-422
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • lsochronous serial interval production (ISIP) data, as from unpaced finger tapping, exhibit higher order dependencies (drift). This fact has largely been ignored by the timing literature, one reason probably being that influential timing models assume ran
  •  
Skapa referenser, mejla, bekava och länka
  • Result 1-31 of 31
Type of publication
journal article (31)
Type of content
peer-reviewed (31)
Author/Editor
Ehrsson, HH (3)
Poom, Leo (3)
Madison, Guy (2)
Madison, Guy, 1961- (2)
Ullén, Fredrik (2)
Lind, Mats (2)
show more...
Johnsrude, Ingrid, 1 ... (2)
Hellström, Åke (1)
Johansson, Petter (1)
Hall, Lars (1)
Burton, G. (1)
von Hofsten, Claes (1)
Terhune, Devin B. (1)
Holsánová, Jana (1)
Berglund, B (1)
Börjesson, Erik (1)
Holmqvist, Kenneth (1)
Olsson, MJ (1)
Johansson, Roger (1)
Marcusson-Clavertz, ... (1)
Marsh, John E. (1)
Andersson, Isabell E ... (1)
Runeson, Sverker, 19 ... (1)
Runeson, Sverker (1)
Olofsson, Jonas K. (1)
Rudner, Mary (1)
Rönnqvist, Louise (1)
Lind, Andreas (1)
Lindskog, Marcus, 19 ... (1)
Baird, JC (1)
Balakrishnan, JD (1)
Ratcliff, R (1)
Juslin, Patrik N, 19 ... (1)
Sörqvist, Patrik (1)
Coppi, S (1)
Lacerda, Francisco (1)
Winman, Anders (1)
Dewhurst, Richard (1)
Patching, Geoffrey (1)
Signoret, Carine (1)
Wykowska, Agnieszka (1)
Lindkvist, Markus (1)
Classon, Elisabet (1)
Collier, Elizabeth S (1)
Lawson, R. (1)
Radziun, D (1)
Hartsuiker, Robert J ... (1)
Holm, Linus (1)
Körning-Ljungberg, J ... (1)
Englund, Mats P., 19 ... (1)
show less...
University
Uppsala University (10)
Karolinska Institutet (7)
Umeå University (5)
Lund University (4)
Stockholm University (3)
Luleå University of Technology (2)
show more...
Linköping University (2)
University of Gävle (1)
Linnaeus University (1)
RISE (1)
show less...
Language
English (31)
Research subject (UKÄ/SCB)
Social Sciences (18)
Natural sciences (2)
Engineering and Technology (1)
Medical and Health Sciences (1)

Year

Kungliga biblioteket hanterar dina personuppgifter i enlighet med EU:s dataskyddsförordning (2018), GDPR. Läs mer om hur det funkar här.
Så här hanterar KB dina uppgifter vid användning av denna tjänst.

 
pil uppåt Close

Copy and save the link in order to return to this view