SwePub
Sök i SwePub databas

  Extended search

Träfflista för sökning "hsv:(SAMHÄLLSVETENSKAP) hsv:(Psykologi) hsv:(Tillämpad psykologi) "

Search: hsv:(SAMHÄLLSVETENSKAP) hsv:(Psykologi) hsv:(Tillämpad psykologi)

  • Result 21-30 of 5089
Sort/group result
   
EnumerationReferenceCoverFind
21.
  • Clausén Gull, Ingela, et al. (author)
  • Neighborhood conditions in a Swedish context - Two studies of reliability and validity of virtual systematic social observation using Google Street View
  • 2023
  • In: Frontiers in Psychology. - : Frontiers Media SA. - 1664-1078. ; 14
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Introduction: The goal of these studies was to investigate the reliability and validity of virtual systematic social observation (virtual SSO) using Google Street View in a Swedish neighborhood context.Methods: This was accomplished in two studies. Study 1 focused on interrater reliability and construct validity, comparing ratings conducted in-person to those done using Google Street View, across 24 study sites within four postal code areas. Study 2 focused on criterion validity of virtual SSO in terms of neighborhoods with low versus high income levels, including 133 study sites within 22 postal code areas in a large Swedish city. In both studies, assessment of the neighborhood context was conducted at each study site, using a protocol adapted to a Swedish context.Results: Scales for Physical Decay, Neighborhood Dangerousness, and Physical Disorder were found to be reliable, with adequate interrater reliability, high consistency across methods, and high internal consistency. In Study 2, significantly higher levels of observed Physical Decay, Neighborhood Dangerousness, and signs of garbage or litter were observed in postal codes areas (site data was aggregated to postal code level) with lower as compared to higher income levels.Discussion: We concluded that the scales within the virtual SSO with Google Street View protocol that were developed in this series of studies represents a reliable and valid measure of several key neighborhood contextual features. Implications for understanding the complex person-context interactions central to many theories of positive development among youth were discussed in relation to the study findings.
  •  
22.
  • Costache, Madalina Elena, et al. (author)
  • Higher- and lower-order personality traits and cluster subtypes in social anxiety disorder
  • 2020
  • In: PLOS ONE. - : PUBLIC LIBRARY SCIENCE. - 1932-6203. ; 15:4
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Social anxiety disorder (SAD) can come in different forms, presenting problems for diagnostic classification. Here, we examined personality traits in a large sample of patients (N = 265) diagnosed with SAD in comparison to healthy controls (N = 164) by use of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and Karolinska Scales of Personality (KSP). In addition, we identified subtypes of SAD based on cluster analysis of the NEO-PI-R Big Five personality dimensions. Significant group differences in personality traits between patients and controls were noted on all Big Five dimensions except agreeableness. Group differences were further noted on most lower-order facets of NEO-PI-R, and nearly all KSP variables. A logistic regression analysis showed, however, that only neuroticism and extraversion remained significant independent predictors of patient/control group when controlling for the effects of the other Big Five dimensions. Also, only neuroticism and extraversion yielded large effect sizes when SAD patients were compared to Swedish normative data for the NEO-PI-R. A two-step cluster analysis resulted in three separate clusters labelled Prototypical (33%), Introvert-Conscientious (29%), and Instable-Open (38%) SAD. Individuals in the Prototypical cluster deviated most on the Big Five dimensions and they were at the most severe end in profile analyses of social anxiety, self-rated fear during public speaking, trait anxiety, and anxiety-related KSP variables. While additional studies are needed to determine if personality subtypes in SAD differ in etiological and treatment-related factors, the present results demonstrate considerable personality heterogeneity in socially anxious individuals, further underscoring that SAD is a multidimensional disorder.
  •  
23.
  • Dahl, Mats, et al. (author)
  • Old and very old adults as witnesses: event memory and metamemory
  • 2015
  • In: Psychology, Crime and Law. - 1477-2744 .- 1068-316X. ; 21:8, s. 764-775
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Older people constitute an important category of eyewitnesses. Episodic memory performance in older persons is poorer than in younger adults, but little research has been made on older persons' metacognitive judgments. Since more persons of advanced age will likely be called upon as witnesses in coming years, it is critical to characterize this population's metacognitive abilities. We compared event memory metacognition in old adults (66-year-old, n = 74) to very old adults (87 or 90 years old, n = 55). Participants were tested on their memory of a film, using questions with two answer alternatives and the confidence in their answer. As expected, the very old group had a lower accuracy rate than the old group (d = 0.59). The very old group, however, monitored this impairment, since their over-/underconfidence and calibration did not differ from the old group but they displayed a poorer ability to separate correct from incorrect answers (discrimination ability). Possibly, the very old group was able to monitor the level of their over-/underconfidence because they applied general self-knowledge about their memory skills. In contrast, the discrimination of correct from incorrect answers may be more dependent on ability to attend to the features of each retrieved memory.
  •  
24.
  • Dahl, Mats, et al. (author)
  • The relation between realism in confidence judgements and the phenomenological quality of recognition memory when using emotionally valenced pictures
  • 2006
  • In: Applied Cognitive Psychology. - : John Wiley and Sons Ltd. - 0888-4080 .- 1099-0720. ; 20:6, s. 791-806
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • The aim of the study was to investigate the relation between the phenomenological quality of memory (Tulving, 1985) and the realism (validity) in confidence judgement when using emotional pictures (I.A.P.S; Lang, Ohman, & Vaitl, 1988). A series of three experiments was completed where the participants judged the phenomenological quality of their memory and/or their confidence. The results showed facilitation for the negative pictures in a matrix search task in the encoding phase, where negative pictures were more easily and quickly detected, compared to positive ones. In the memory phase of the experiments a higher degree of recollective experience (a larger proportion of 'remember' responses) was found for negative pictures. A higher level of confidence for recognition of negative pictures than for positive ones was obtained, but no general valence dependent effect on the realism in the confidence judgement was found. However, when analysing only the remember responses, negative pictures showed higher overconfidence than the positive pictures. The results support that a recollective experience induces higher confidence and overconfidence.
  •  
25.
  •  
26.
  • Eriksson, Kimmo, et al. (author)
  • A New Look at Individual Differences in Perceptions of Unfairness : The Theory of Maximally Unfair Allocations in Multiparty Situations
  • 2015
  • In: Social Justice Research. - New York : Springer Science+Business Media B.V.. - 0885-7466 .- 1573-6725. ; 28:4, s. 401-414
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Previous research has demonstrated that unfairness judgments of resource allocations become more complex when there are more than two recipients. In order to explain some of this complexity, we propose a set of psychological mechanisms that may underlie four different choices of maximally unfair resource allocations (MUA): Self-Single-Loser, Self-One-Loser-of-Many, Self-Single-Winner, and Self-One-Winner-of-Many. From this psychological theory, several predictions are derived and tested in vignette studies involving a total of 708 participants recruited online using MTurk. As predicted by our theory, (1) choices of MUA where there is a single loser were much more common when the allocated resource was of negative rather than positive valence, and (2) the amount of egoistic bias individuals exhibited when judging the unfairness in receiving a small rather than a large share in a non-extreme multi-party allocation was predicted by their choices of MUA. These findings suggest that an individual’s choice of MUA reveals some generally relevant principles of how unfairness is perceived in multi-party allocations. This opens up new lines of inquiry, especially regarding research on social dilemmas and social value orientation.
  •  
27.
  • Garcia, Danilo, 1973, et al. (author)
  • The 18-item Swedish version of Ryff's psychological wellbeing scale: psychometric properties based on classical test theory and item response theory
  • 2023
  • In: Frontiers in Psychology. - : FRONTIERS MEDIA SA. - 1664-1078. ; 14
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • BackgroundPsychological wellbeing is conceptualized as the full engagement and optimal performance in existential challenges of life. Our understanding of psychological wellbeing is important for us humans to survive, adapt, and thrive during the challenges of the 21st century. Hence, the measurement of psychological wellbeing is one cornerstone for the identification and treatment of both mental illness and health promotion. In this context, Ryff operationalized psychological wellbeing as a six-dimensional model of human characteristics: self-acceptance, positive relations with others, environmental mastery, personal growth, autonomy, and purpose in life. Ryff's Psychological Wellbeing Scale has been developed and translated into different versions. Here, we examine and describe the psychometric properties of the 18-item Swedish version of Ryff's Psychological Wellbeing Scale using both Classical Test Theory (CTT) and Item Response Theory (IRT).MethodsThe data used in the present study was earlier published elsewhere and consists of 768 participants (279 women and 489 men). In addition to the 18-item version of the scale, participants answered the Temporal Satisfaction with Life Scale, the Positive Affect Negative Affect Schedule, and the Background and Health Questionnaire. We examined, the 18-item version's factor structure using different models and its relationship with subjective wellbeing, sociodemographic factors (e.g., education level, gender, age), lifestyle habits (i.e., smoking, frequency of doing exercise, and exercise intensity), and health issues (i.e., pain and sleeping problems). We also analyzed measurement invariance with regard to gender. Moreover, as an addition to the existing literature, we analyzed the properties of the 18 items using Graded Response Model (GRM).ResultsAlthough the original six-factor structure showed a good fit, both CTT and IRT indicated that a five-factor model, without the purpose in life subscale, provided a better fit. The results supported the internal consistency and concurrent validity of the 18-item Swedish version. Moreover, invariance testing showed similar measurement precision by the scale across gender. Finally, we found several items, especially the purpose in life's item "I live life one day at a time and do not really think about the future," that might need revision or modification in order to improve measurement.ConclusionA five-factor solution is a valid and reliable measure for the assessment of psychological wellbeing in the general Swedish population. With some modifications, the scale might achieve enough accuracy to measure the more appropriate and correct six-dimensional theoretical framework as detailed by Ryff. Fortunately, Ryff's original version contains 20 items per subscale and should therefore act as a perfect pool of items in this endeavor.
  •  
28.
  • Geurts, Renate, 1984, et al. (author)
  • Interviewing to manage threats: Exploring the effects of interview style on information gain and threateners’ counter-interview strategies
  • 2018
  • In: Journal of Threat Assessment and Management. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 2169-4850 .- 2169-4842. ; 5:4, s. 189-204
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • There is consensus about the importance to engage with, and if possible interview, individuals who threaten to cause harm. However, there exist little research on how to conduct such interviews. This paper contributes with an experimental approach on threat management interviewing. We explored what types of counter-interview strategies threateners employ, and we tested the efficacy of two common interview styles (direct interviewing vs. rapport-based interviewing). Participants (N = 120) were interviewed about a non-violent threat they had made (to press charges against their former employer) and reported what strategies they had used during the interview. No differences were found between the interview protocols for threat management outcomes (i.e., information gain, use of counter-interview strategies, and willingness to discuss or enact the threat). However, the study showed how threateners struck a deliberate balance between proving their stand and disguising implementation details. Critically, individuals with more serious intentions to enact the threat were more inclined to hide information from the interviewer. We argue that it is vital for threat management interviewers to i) understand what behaviors can be expected from the interviewee, and ii) learn about interview methods that can steer these behaviors towards information gain (which is beneficial to threat assessment) and towards de-escalation (which is the purpose of threat management).
  •  
29.
  • Hagsand, Angelica, 1985, et al. (author)
  • Alcohol, crime and memory. Intoxicated eyewitnesses delayed recall of a kidnapping.
  • 2012
  • In: Svenska föreningen för Alkohol- och Drogforskning, konferens 8-9 November, Norrköping.
  • Conference paper (other academic/artistic)abstract
    • Alcohol is involved in 50-70% of violent crimes in Sweden. Eyewitness memory is a valuable source in investigations and it is common that the police interview alcohol intoxicated eyewitnesses. There are few studies on how alcohol affects witness memory. This study investigated how different doses of alcohol affected eyewitness recall one week after witnessing a crime and potential sex differences. The participants (N = 126) were healthy adults and were randomly assigned to either a control group, 0.0 g/kg ethanol (N = 42), a lower alcohol dose group, 0.4 g/kg ethanol (N = 40), or a higher alcohol dose group, 0.7 g/kg ethanol (N = 44). After 15 minutes consumption in a laboratory, participants witnessed a film showing a kidnapping of a woman by two men. The witnesses were interviewed about the crime one week later in a sober state. Witnesses in the higher alcohol dose group recalled fewer details compared to witnesses in the lower alcohol dose group. The amount of alcohol consumed did not have an impact on accuracy. Women and men reached the same blood alcohol concentration and no sex differences were found in recall. Interestingly, although the witnesses in the high alcohol dose group reported less information, their testimony was as correct as the testimony given by witnesses in the control group and the lower alcohol dose group. Despite the interesting results, more studies are needed before recommendations to the legal system can be made.
  •  
30.
  • Hagsand, Angelica, 1985, et al. (author)
  • Alcohol intoxicated eyewitnesses´ delayed recall of a kidnapping.
  • 2013
  • In: Poster presented at the European Association of Psychology and Law, 5th of September 2013, Coventry, UK..
  • Conference paper (other academic/artistic)abstract
    • This study investigated how different doses of alcohol affected eyewitness recall. Participants (N = 126) were randomly assigned to three groups with different blood alcohol concentration (BAC), either a control group (mean BAC 0.00%, N = 42), a lower alcohol dose group (mean BAC 0.04%, N = 40), or a higher alcohol dose group (mean BAC 0.06%, N = 44). After consumption in a laboratory, participants witnessed a film of a mock crime where a woman was kidnapped by two men. One week after, the witnesses were interviewed in a sober state, by interviewers who were blind to which beverage the witnesses had consumed the week before. The main results showed that witnesses with the higher intoxication level recalled fewer details compared to witnesses with the lower intoxication level. The amount of alcohol consumed did not have an impact on the accuracy rate. No sex differences were found. We conclude that more studies are needed before recommendations can be made to an applied setting, but this study showed that alcohol may have a negative impact on eyewitness recall.
  •  
Skapa referenser, mejla, bekava och länka
  • Result 21-30 of 5089
Type of publication
journal article (2917)
conference paper (958)
book chapter (358)
reports (331)
doctoral thesis (125)
research review (108)
show more...
other publication (99)
book (89)
licentiate thesis (38)
editorial proceedings (36)
editorial collection (22)
review (8)
artistic work (4)
show less...
Type of content
peer-reviewed (3558)
other academic/artistic (1412)
pop. science, debate, etc. (118)
Author/Editor
Andersson, Gerhard (147)
Carlbring, Per (109)
Stambulova, Natalia, ... (99)
Landström, Sara, 198 ... (98)
Granhag, Pär-Anders, ... (93)
Edvardsson, Bo, 1944 ... (82)
show more...
Holmes, Emily A. (68)
Andersson, Gerhard, ... (68)
Hesser, Hugo, 1982- (68)
Johansson Hanse, Jan ... (60)
Trindade, Inês A., 1 ... (58)
Strömwall, Leif, 196 ... (54)
Ask, Karl, 1978 (53)
Kenttä, Göran, 1965- (52)
Kaldo, Viktor (49)
Ljótsson, Brjánn (48)
Ferreira, Cláudia (47)
Fridell, Mats (44)
Nordin-Bates, Sanna ... (44)
Törner, Marianne, 19 ... (41)
Madison, Guy (40)
Winkel, Jörgen, 1946 (40)
Pienaar, Jaco (40)
Kircher, Katja, 1973 ... (39)
Andersson, Gerhard, ... (39)
Johansson, Maria (39)
Andersson, Gerhard, ... (39)
Gärling, Tommy, 1941 (38)
Ernberg, Emelie, 198 ... (38)
McCracken, Lance, 19 ... (37)
Ivarsson, Andreas, 1 ... (36)
Lindefors, Nils (36)
Ahlström, Christer, ... (35)
Hagsand, Angelica, 1 ... (35)
Pousette, Anders, 19 ... (34)
Johnson, Urban, 1961 ... (32)
Rozental, Alexander (32)
Andersson, Erik (31)
Larsman, Pernilla, 1 ... (31)
Sorbring, Emma, 1972 ... (30)
Roos Af Hjelmsäter, ... (30)
Pinto-Gouveia, José (30)
Hesser, Hugo (29)
Anund, Anna (28)
Sverke, Magnus (28)
Jarebrant, Caroline (28)
Stenling, Andreas, 1 ... (28)
Kenttä, Göran (28)
Falkenström, Fredrik (27)
Axberg, Ulf, 1961 (27)
show less...
University
University of Gothenburg (1050)
Linköping University (728)
Karolinska Institutet (655)
Uppsala University (534)
Stockholm University (481)
Umeå University (422)
show more...
Lund University (414)
VTI - The Swedish National Road and Transport Research Institute (401)
Örebro University (392)
Chalmers University of Technology (318)
Linnaeus University (273)
Halmstad University (209)
The Swedish School of Sport and Health Sciences (199)
University West (183)
Karlstad University (171)
University of Gävle (166)
Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences (124)
Mid Sweden University (108)
Mälardalen University (81)
University of Skövde (79)
Jönköping University (78)
Luleå University of Technology (61)
Royal Institute of Technology (60)
Swedish National Defence College (49)
Marie Cederschiöld högskola (44)
Malmö University (36)
Kristianstad University College (25)
University of Borås (24)
RISE (21)
Högskolan Dalarna (21)
Södertörn University (20)
Blekinge Institute of Technology (17)
Sophiahemmet University College (11)
Stockholm School of Economics (8)
Red Cross University College (5)
University College of Arts, Crafts and Design (4)
Royal College of Music (4)
Stockholm University of the Arts (2)
University College Stockholm (1)
show less...
Language
English (4326)
Swedish (742)
German (6)
Norwegian (6)
Spanish (2)
Finnish (2)
show more...
Portuguese (2)
French (1)
Danish (1)
Arabic (1)
show less...
Research subject (UKÄ/SCB)
Social Sciences (5084)
Medical and Health Sciences (1158)
Engineering and Technology (496)
Natural sciences (184)
Humanities (153)
Agricultural Sciences (63)

Year

Kungliga biblioteket hanterar dina personuppgifter i enlighet med EU:s dataskyddsförordning (2018), GDPR. Läs mer om hur det funkar här.
Så här hanterar KB dina uppgifter vid användning av denna tjänst.

 
pil uppåt Close

Copy and save the link in order to return to this view