SwePub
Sök i SwePub databas

  Utökad sökning

Träfflista för sökning "AMNE:(HUMANIORA Språk och litteratur) ;pers:(Gullberg Marianne)"

Sökning: AMNE:(HUMANIORA Språk och litteratur) > Gullberg Marianne

  • Resultat 1-10 av 127
Sortera/gruppera träfflistan
   
NumreringReferensOmslagsbildHitta
1.
  •  
2.
  • Williams, John N., et al. (författare)
  • Cognitive second language acquisition: An overview
  • 2012
  • Ingår i: Cognitive second language acquisition (The Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics; Ed. C. Chapelle). - Oxford, UK : Blackwell Publishing Ltd. - 9781405198431 ; , s. 1-4
  • Bokkapitel (refereegranskat)
  •  
3.
  • Andersson, Annika, 1966-, et al. (författare)
  • An ERP study of the relationship between verb semantics and events
  • 2016
  • Ingår i: The 8th Annual Meeting of the Society for the Neurobiology of Language.
  • Konferensbidrag (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Languages differ in how events are described, but little is known about how semantics interacts with online event processing. This study targets this question examining placement events in Swedish. Swedish has three obligatory placement verbs for events where objects have support from below: sätta ’set’, ställa ’stand’, and lägga ’lay’. Swedish lacks a superordinate general term like English put (Gullberg & Burenhult, 2011). For every placement event the verb choice depends on object properties, and the direction of the object’s extension from the ground. We use event-related potentials (ERPs) and appropriateness ratings of verb usage to investigate the interaction between verb semantics and event properties. Typically violations of semantic congruency positively affect the amplitude of the N400 (Kutas & Hillyard, 1980). Studies also report a centro-parietal positivity (P600) when real-world knowledge is violated and verbs are incongruous to preceding contexts (Kuperberg, 2007, for a review). Previous ERP studies of visually presented images or movies of actions and events have reported an N400 followed by a P600 when the function of an object is violated (e.g., using a screwdriver as a key, Bach, et al., 2009; Balconi & Caldiroli, 2011).Method: Native speakers (N = 24, 18-35 years) watched still images of placement events followed by sentences visually presented word by word. Sentences described the depicted events while ERPs were recorded and time-locked to the placement verbs. Participants also did an appropriateness rating offline. Object properties (Base/Without base), symmetry (Sym/Asym), and orientation from the ground (Vertical/Horizontal) were varied and sentences with the three different placement verbs were combined with each image in a cross-subject design.Results: Base was important for appropriateness ratings of verb usage with symmetric objects while orientation was important for asymmetric objects. In contrast, there were no ERP effects to base (Base/Without) for symmetric objects. Asymmetric-base objects showed increased N400s and P600s with verbs incongruent with the depicted events (orientation, e.g., ‘lay’ with vertical glass). Asymmetric-Without base elicited an increased P600 when verbs were incongruent to depicted events when horizontally oriented (e.g., ‘set’ with horizontal avocado), but an increased N400 when verbs were incongruent to the atypical vertical placement of the objects (e.g., ‘lay’ with a vertical avocado).Discussion: Results showed an increased amplitude of both ERP effects (N400/P600) when placement verbs were incongruent with typical placement scenarios of objects that in the real-world are placed vertically or horizontally (Asymmetric-Base, e.g., a candle; cf. Bach et al., 2009). However, for objects without a base the anterior negativity was increased with a mismatch between the verb and the presented images (the depicted events), while the P600 increased for mismatches between the verb and typical real-world events. These results suggest the anterior N400 and the P600 indeed index different relationships with event processing as previously suggested for images (Sitnikova, et al., 2008). Our results agree with previous studies suggesting that the processing of verb meaning in language cannot be separated from knowledge of object handling in the real world (cf., Van Berkum, et al., 2008).
  •  
4.
  • Andrén, Mats, et al. (författare)
  • Att lära sig språk
  • 2013
  • Ingår i: Språket, människan och världen : människans språk 1-2 - människans språk 1-2. - Lund : Studentlitteratur AB. - 9789144083391 ; , s. 73-89
  • Bokkapitel (populärvet., debatt m.m.)
  •  
5.
  • Sayehli, Susan, et al. (författare)
  • Native Word Order Processing Is Not Uniform : An ERP Study of Verb-Second Word Order
  • 2022
  • Ingår i: Frontiers in Psychology. - : Frontiers Media S.A.. - 1664-1078. ; 13, s. 1-22
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Studies of native syntactic processing often target phrase structure violations that do not occur in natural production. In contrast, this study examines how variation in basic word order is processed, looking specifically at structures traditionally labelled as violations but that do occur naturally. We examined Swedish verb-second (V2) and verb-third (V3) word order processing in adult native Swedish speakers, manipulating sentence-initial adverbials (temporal idag ‘today’, spatial hemma ‘at home’ and sentential kanske ‘maybe’) in acceptability judgements, in simultaneously recorded event-related potentials (ERP) to visually presented sentences and in a written sentence completion task. An initial corpus study showed that the adverbials differ in frequency in fronted position (idag > kanske > hemma), and although all occur mainly with V2 word order, kanske occurs more frequently with V3 in natural production than both idag and hemma. The experimental results reflected these patterns such that V2 sentences were overall more frequently produced and were deemed more acceptable than V3 sentences. The ERP results consisted of a biphasic N400/P600 response to V3 word order that indicated effects of word retrieval and sentence reanalysis. We also found consistent effects of adverbials. As predicted, V3 was produced more frequently and judged as more acceptable in Kanske sentences than in sentences with the other two adverbials. The ERP analyses showed stronger effects for idag and hemma with V3, especially regarding the P600. The results suggest that the naturally occurring word order ‘violation’, V3 with kanske, is processed differently than V3 with other adverbials where the V2 norm is stronger. Moreover, these patterns are related to individuals’ own production patterns. Overall, the results suggest a more varied native word order processing than previously reported.
  •  
6.
  • Andersson, Annika, 1966-, et al. (författare)
  • Ditt förstaspråks grammatik påverkar hur din hjärna bearbetar dina andraspråk
  • 2015
  • Ingår i: Grammatikdagen 2015, Lund.
  • Konferensbidrag (övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt)abstract
    • Neurokognitiva studier av hjärnans bearbetning av första- och andraspråksgrammatik relaterar skillnader till inlärningsålder och andraspråksfärdighet. Dessa studier undersöker typiskt inte hur skillnader och likheter i språkens strukturer påverkar bearbetningen. Vi utforskade hur andraspråkstalare med goda språkfärdigheter i Svenska bearbetade svenskans verbplacering beroende på om deras första språk hade samma verbplacering som svenskan (tyska) eller inte (engelska). Denna studie visar att hjärnans bearbetning av ett andra språk inte kan begränsas till en diskussion om ålder vid inlärande eller språkfärdighet.
  •  
7.
  •  
8.
  • Andersson, Annika, 1966-, et al. (författare)
  • Language background affects word order processing in a second language online but not offline
  • 2014
  • Ingår i: Culture, Brain, Learning : Wallenberg Network Initiative. - Lund : Lund University. ; , s. 15-15
  • Konferensbidrag (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Different languages organize information differently, for example in different word orders. A large body of work shows that learning to use word order in a new, second language (L2) is difficult. An example is the production of verb-second (V2) word order, which requires the finite verb in main clauses to appear in second position even when the sentence does not start with a subject. V2 difficulties are ubiquitous and only partially modulated by patterns in thefirst language (L1; e.g., Ganuza, 2008 for an overview). Despite the body of work on L2 production, we know surprisingly little about how word order is processed behaviorally and neurocognitively, and how production relates to comprehension. This study therefore examined how advanced German (n=14) and English (n=14) adult learners, matched for proficiency and age of acquisition, process word order in Swedish compared to native speakers (n=20) depending on L1 background (i.e., ±similar word order in the L1; German [+V2] vs. English [-V2]), sentence-initial adverb frequency (frequent idag ‘today’ vs. infrequent hemma ‘at home’ (1)), and length of the sentence-initial constituent (short vs. long prefield; (2)).(1) Idag/Hemma läste hon tidningen. vs. *Idag/Hemma hon läste tidningen. Today/At home read she paper.def vs. *Today/At home she read paper.def(2) Idag/Hemma hos Maria läste hon tidningen. vs. *Idag/Hemma hos Maria hon läste tidningen. Today/At home at Maria’s read she paper.def vs. *Today/At home at Maria’s she read paper.def.We probed the production of word order in a sentence completion task and examined responses to word order (violations) in a timed acceptability judgment task during which participants were presented with sentences word by word while event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded. At the end of the sentence participants judged the sentence acceptability.Overall, the results indicated that the two learner groups behaved similarly on behavioral measures of comprehension and production, but crucially differed in online processing. All groups, including learners, showed sensitivity to V2-violations in the ERPs. Swedish native speakers were also sensitive to length of prefield showing the typical biphasic ERP response only to violations with long prefields allowing build up of expectations. Importantly, the learners, who did not differ behaviorally, showed different responses. The German learners [+V2] showed similar ERP patterns to native Swedish speakers, whereas the English learners [-V2] showed more variation in their ERP responses. We discuss these findings in terms of theories of crosslinguistic influence and theories of native-like syntacticprocessing.
  •  
9.
  • Andersson, Annika, 1966-, et al. (författare)
  • Language background affects online word order processing in a second language but not offline
  • 2019
  • Ingår i: Bilingualism. - : Cambridge University Press. - 1366-7289 .- 1469-1841. ; 22:4, s. 802-825
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • This study examines possible crosslinguistic influence on basic word order processing in a second language (L2). Targeting Swedish V2 word order we investigate adult German learners (+V2 in the L1) and English learners (-V2 in the L1) of Swedish who are matched for proficiency. We report results from two offline behavioural tasks (written production, metalinguistic judgments), and online processing as measured by event-related potentials (ERPs). All groups showed sensitivity to word order violations behaviourally and neurocognitively. Behaviourally, the learners differed from the native speakers only on judgements. Crucially, they did not differ from each other. Neurocognitively, all groups showed a similar increased centro-parietal P600 ERP-effect, but German learners (+V2) displayed more nativelike anterior ERP-effects than English learners (-V2). The results suggest crosslinguistic influence in that the presence of a similar word order in the L1 can facilitate online processing in an L2-- even if no offline behavioural effects are discerned.
  •  
10.
  • Sayehli, Susan, et al. (författare)
  • Today read she the paper: An ERP study of the processing of word order in Swedish L2
  • 2014
  • Ingår i: Eurosla 24. - York : European Second Language Association. ; , s. 46-46
  • Konferensbidrag (refereegranskat)abstract
    • There is ample evidence that word order is a problematic domain in L2 usage. In particular, production of the verb-second (V2) phenomenon, which requires the finite verb in main clauses to appear in second position, (e.g., Ganuza, 2008 for an overview). Surprisingly, we know very little about how these structures are processed and how production relates to comprehension. We therefore examined how advanced German (N=14) and English (N=14) adult learners, matched for proficiency and age of acquisition (AoA; German M=22, English M=23), process word order in Swedish compared to native speakers (N=20) depending on language background (L1 with [German] or without [English] V2), preposed adverb frequency (frequent idag ‘today’ vs. infrequent hemma ‘at home’, ex. 1), and the length of the preposed constituent (short vs. long prefield, ex. 2). (1) Idag/Hemma läste hon tidningen. vs. *Idag/Hemma hon läste tidningen. Today/At home read she paper.def vs. *Today/At home she read paper.def (2) Idag/Hemma hos Maria läste hon tidningen. vs. *Idag/Hemma hos Maria hon läste tidningen. Today/At home at Maria read she paper.def vs. *Today/At home at Maria she read paper.def. We examined responses to word order violations in an acceptability judgement task and an ERP experiment, and probed the production of word order in a sentence completion task. Preliminary results from the judgment task indicated that native speakers were faster and more accurate on judging sentences than both L2 groups who did not differ. Overall, the more frequent adverb, idag, also affected accuracy and reaction times positively, but there were no interactions with group. The outcome from the sentence completion task showed similar results: native speakers were more accurate than the L2 groups who did not differ, and an overall adverb frequency effect was found, but not difference across groups. In contrast, the ERP data showed different patterns. In native speakers V2 violations elicited a bimodal ERP response, an anterior negativity followed by a posterior P600. These effects were increased in amplitude and the anterior negativity was left lateralized (LAN) when the prefield was long. In the German group a bimodal response was detected only when V2 violations followed a frequent adverb in a long prefield. In other cases only a posterior P600 was evident. The English group, in contrast, showed an early anterior positivity, and a later lateral parietal negativity in the N400 time window that was followed by a posterior P600. These responses were affected only by prefield length and only in amplitude. Overall, the results indicated that advanced German and English learners, matched on proficiency and AoA, who performed similarly on behavioural measures of comprehension and production of word order, still differed in online processing. More specifically, language background mattered since the German learners whose L1 share V2 with target Swedish, overall showed similar ERP patterns to native speakers. In contrast, the English learners, whose L1 does not share V2, showed more variation in their ERP responses. We discuss the implications of these findings for theories of crosslinguistic influence and theories of nativelike syntactic processing.
  •  
Skapa referenser, mejla, bekava och länka
  • Resultat 1-10 av 127
Typ av publikation
tidskriftsartikel (54)
bokkapitel (29)
konferensbidrag (21)
annan publikation (14)
samlingsverk (redaktörskap) (5)
doktorsavhandling (3)
visa fler...
recension (1)
visa färre...
Typ av innehåll
refereegranskat (97)
övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt (22)
populärvet., debatt m.m. (8)
Författare/redaktör
Andersson, Annika, 1 ... (11)
Sayehli, Susan (9)
Andersson, Annika (7)
Granfelt, Jonas (6)
Holmqvist, Kenneth (5)
visa fler...
Seyfeddinipur, Manda ... (4)
Brown, Amanda (4)
Narasimhan, Bhuvana (4)
Graziano, Maria (4)
Håkansson, Gisela (3)
Arbona, Eléonore (3)
Arndt, Henriette L. (3)
Padden, Carol (3)
Sandler, Wendy (3)
Johansson, Victoria (2)
Carling, Gerd (2)
van de Weijer, Joost (2)
Holmer, Arthur (2)
Gulz, Agneta (2)
Blomberg, Frida (2)
Andrén, Mats (2)
Cienki, Alan (2)
Nirme, Jens (2)
Haake, Magnus (2)
Seeber, Kilian G. (2)
Burenhult, Niclas (2)
Moren, Jan (2)
Kopecka, Anetta (2)
Chapelle, Carol (2)
Lundell, Patrik (1)
Nyström, Marcus (1)
Muller, Cornelia (1)
House, David (1)
Becker, W. (1)
Niehorster, Diederic ... (1)
Alferink, Inge (1)
Fricke, Ellen (1)
McNeill, David (1)
Blomberg, Johan (1)
Seeber, Kilian (1)
Arndt, Henriette (1)
Morgenstern, Aliyah (1)
Majid, Asifa (1)
Viberg, Åke, Profess ... (1)
Roberts, Leah (1)
Berghoff, Robyn (1)
Kootstra, Gerrit Jan (1)
Shumskaya, Elena (1)
Yager, Joanne (1)
visa färre...
Lärosäte
Lunds universitet (116)
Linnéuniversitetet (13)
Högskolan Kristianstad (2)
Stockholms universitet (2)
Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan (1)
Uppsala universitet (1)
visa fler...
Högskolan i Gävle (1)
Linköpings universitet (1)
Malmö universitet (1)
visa färre...
Språk
Engelska (115)
Svenska (12)
Forskningsämne (UKÄ/SCB)
Humaniora (127)
Samhällsvetenskap (8)
Medicin och hälsovetenskap (2)
Naturvetenskap (1)
Teknik (1)

År

Kungliga biblioteket hanterar dina personuppgifter i enlighet med EU:s dataskyddsförordning (2018), GDPR. Läs mer om hur det funkar här.
Så här hanterar KB dina uppgifter vid användning av denna tjänst.

 
pil uppåt Stäng

Kopiera och spara länken för att återkomma till aktuell vy