SwePub
Sök i SwePub databas

  Utökad sökning

Träfflista för sökning "LAR1:hkr ;lar1:(lnu)"

Sökning: LAR1:hkr > Linnéuniversitetet

  • Resultat 1-10 av 217
Sortera/gruppera träfflistan
   
NumreringReferensOmslagsbildHitta
1.
  • Agevall, Lena, et al. (författare)
  • The new generation of auditors meeting praxis : dual learning's role in audit students' professional development
  • 2018
  • Ingår i: Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research. - : Taylor & Francis. - 0031-3831 .- 1470-1170. ; 62:2, s. 307-324
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • This paper explores whether and in what way “dual learning” can develop understanding of the relationship between structure/judgement and explores audit student’s perceptions of the audit profession. The Work Integrated Learning (WIL) module, serving as a tool of enabling dual learning, represents the context for this exploration. The study is based on a focus group and individual interviews conducted with students performing their WIL. Our data and its analysis indicates that when in a WIL context, students develop awareness of the use of standards and checklists on the one hand, and the importance of discretional judgement on the other. Based on these results, we theorise as to how dual learning manifests itself in students’ experiences and understanding of the relationship between structure and judgement.
  •  
2.
  • Ahlberg, Jenny, et al. (författare)
  • Board functions in governance arenas : a comparative case study of four Swedish family firms
  • 2023
  • Ingår i: Journal of Family Business Management. - : Emerald Group Publishing Limited. - 2043-6238 .- 2043-6246. ; 14:3, s. 672-696
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Purpose The purpose of this paper is to explore board functions and their location in family firms.Design/methodology/approach Through structured induction in a four-case study of medium-sized Swedish family firms, the authors demonstrate that board functions can be located in other arenas than in the common board and suggest propositions that explain their distribution. Findings (1) The board is but one of several arenas where board functions are performed. (2) The functions performed by the board vary in type and emphasis. (3) The non-family directors in a family firm serve the owners, even sometimes governing them, in what the authors term "bidirectional governance". (4) The kin strategy of the family influences their governance. (5) The utilization of a board for governance stems from the family (together with its constitution, kin strategy and governance strategy), the board composition and the business conditions of the firm.Research limitations/implications Being a case study the findings are restricted to concepts and theoretical propositions. Using structured induction, the study is not solely inductive but still contains the subjectivity of induction.Practical implications Governance agents should have an instrumental view on the board, considering it one possible governance arena among others, thereby economizing on governance.Social implications The institutional pressure toward active boards could paradoxically reduce the importance of the board in family firms.Originality/value The board of a family company differs in its emphasis of board functions and these functions are performed with varying emphases in different governance arenas. The authors propose the concept of kin strategy, which refers to the governance importance of the structure of the owner and observations on bi-directional governance, indicating that the board can govern the owners.
  •  
3.
  • Airey, John, 1963-, et al. (författare)
  • On the Disciplinary Affordances of Semiotic Resources
  • 2014
  • Ingår i: Book of Abstracts. ; , s. 54-55, s. 54-55
  • Konferensbidrag (refereegranskat)abstract
    • In the late 70’s Gibson (1979) introduced the concept of affordance. Initially framed around the needs of an organism in its environment, over the years the term has been appropriated and debated at length by a number of researchers in various fields. Most famous, perhaps is the disagreement between Gibson and Norman (1988) about whether affordances are inherent properties of objects or are only present when they are perceived by an organism. More recently, affordance has been drawn on in the educational arena, particularly with respect to multimodality (see Linder (2013) for a recent example). Here, Kress et al. (2001) have claimed that different modes have different specialized affordances. Then, building on this idea, Airey and Linder (2009) suggested that there is a critical constellation of modes that students need to achieve fluency in before they can experience a concept in an appropriate disciplinary manner. Later, Airey (2009) nuanced this claim, shifting the focus from the modes themselves to a critical constellation of semiotic resources, thus acknowledging that different semiotic resources within a mode often have different affordances (e.g. two or more diagrams may form the critical constellation).In this theoretical paper the concept of disciplinary affordance (Fredlund et al., 2012) is suggested as a useful analytical tool for use in education. The concept makes a radical break with the views of both Gibson and Norman in that rather than focusing on the discernment of one individual, it refers to the disciplinary community as a whole. Put simply, the disciplinary affordances of a given semiotic resource are determined by those functions that the resource is expected to fulfil by the disciplinary community. Disciplinary affordances have thus been negotiated and developed within the discipline over time. As such, the question of whether these affordances are inherent or discerned becomes moot. Rather, from an educational perspective the issue is whether the meaning that a semiotic resource affords to an individual matches the disciplinary affordance assigned by the community. The power of the term for educational work is that learning can now be framed as coming to discern the disciplinary affordances of semiotic resources.In this paper we will briefly discuss the history of the term affordance, define the term disciplinary affordance and illustrate its usefulness in a number of educational settings.
  •  
4.
  • Airey, John, 1963-, et al. (författare)
  • What do you see here? : Using an analysis of the Hertzsprung-Russell diagram in astronomy to create a survey of disciplinary discernment.
  • 2014
  • Ingår i: The first Conference of the International Association for Cognitive Semiotics, Lund, Sweden, 25-27 Sept 2014. ; , s. 52-53
  • Konferensbidrag (refereegranskat)abstract
    • Becoming part of a discipline involves learning to interpret and use a range of disciplinary-specific semiotic resources (Airey, 2009). These resources have been developed and assigned particular specialist meanings over time. Nowhere is this truer than in the sciences, where it is the norm that disciplinary-specific representations have been introduced and then refined by a number of different actors in order to reconcile them with subsequent empirical and theoretical advances. As a consequence, many of the semiotic resources used in the sciences today still retain some (potentially confusing) traces of their historical roots. However, it has been repeatedly shown that university lecturers underestimate the challenges such disciplinary specific semiotic resources may present to undergraduates (Northedge, 2002; Tobias, 1986).In this paper we analyse one such disciplinary-specific semiotic resource from the field of Astronomy—the Hertzsprung-Russell diagram. First, we audit the potential of this semiotic resource to provide access to disciplinary knowledge—what Fredlund et al (2012) have termed its disciplinary affordances. Our analysis includes consideration of the use of scales, labels, symbols, sizes and colour. We show how, for historical reasons, the use of these aspects in the resource may differ from what might be expected by a newcomer to the discipline. Using the results of our analysis we then created an online questionnaire to probe what is discerned (Eriksson, Linder, Airey, & Redfors, in press) with respect to each of these aspects by astronomers and physicists ranging from first year undergraduates to university professors.Our findings suggest that some of the issues we highlight in our analysis may, in fact, be contributors to the alternative conceptions of undergraduate students and we therefore propose that lecturers pay particular attention to the disambiguation of these features for their students.
  •  
5.
  • Akner, Gunnar, 1953-, et al. (författare)
  • Multisjuka och bräckliga äldre
  • 2015. - 1
  • Ingår i: Mat och hälsa. - Stockholm : Studentlitteratur AB. - 9789144083575 ; , s. 105-108
  • Bokkapitel (övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt)
  •  
6.
  •  
7.
  • Ambrazaitis, Gilbert, 1979-, et al. (författare)
  • Do dialect-specific prosodic properties shape the path to contrastive focus? : Production and comprehension data from 3-5 year-old children acquiring Stockholm or Scania Swedish
  • 2023
  • Konferensbidrag (populärvet., debatt m.m.)abstract
    • Cuing information structure (IS) is a fundamental function of prosody in many languages. For instance, in English or Dutch, a contrastive focus on the color adjective in a phrase like ‘the green boat’ is marked by a pitch accent on GREEN and, crucially, a lack of post-focal accent on BOAT: ‘the GREEN boat’. Listeners infer from this prosodic structure that ‘boat’ represents already activated (given) information and that the color that is specially mentioned represents one out of a set of alternatives. According to research findings, children achieve proficiency in the production of prosodic IS encoding within the age range of 4 to 8 years, displaying considerable variability. This variability is suggested to result from structural prosodic differences between languages. For instance, Stockholm Swedish speaking children mark (non-contrastive) focus using the Swedish prominence cuing H(igh) tone in an adult-like manner already at 4-5 years, while Dutch speaking children handle Dutch intonational pitch accents only after the age of 7-8 years. One hypothesis is that this relates to the presence of lexical pitch accents in Swedish, which could make Swedish speaking children more sensitive to prosodic contrasts; in addition, the combination of lexical accent + prominence H results in a complex contour which is particularly salient. However, studies investigating this have usually had a strict focus on speech production. The few previous studies that have conducted parallel production and comprehension experiments have typically used offline methods to assess comprehension. More recent studies using online methods such as eye tracking have usually not included children younger than 6 years of age and have not been complemented by production data. In this study we combine production and comprehension experiments, using eye tracking, to study contrastive focus prosody in 3- to 5-year-old children speaking either Scanian or Stockholm Swedish. In Scanian, instead of adding the prominence H-tone for focus, phrase-level prominence is encoded through phonetic adjustments of the (lexical) HL accent patterns. By comparing these two Swedish varieties we can thus control for phonological features (incl. lexical tone), as well as grammar and lexicon, when exploring effects of prosodic-typological differences. In our production experiment we elicit adjective-noun phrases in three different focus conditions (broad, contrast on adjective, contrast on noun), using an interactive video/card game. Production data are analyzed acoustically and auditorily. As for comprehension, our visual-word eye-tracking experiment makes use of the same pictures of colored objects to investigate whether and how children rely on prosody for reference resolution (e.g., Where is the yellow boat? And where is the GREEN boat?). The time course of eye movements will be analyzed using growth curves. Both production and eye-tracking data will be analyzed as a function of dialect, age and standardized measures of language production and comprehension (The New Reynell Developmental Language Scales), as well as compared to data from adult controls. Data are currently being collected. A preliminary analysis of eye-tracking data from 24 Scanian children (ages 3-5 years collapsed) and a subset of adults from both dialects suggests similar comprehension of focus prosody as in adults (as a mismatched focus prosody in the adjective successfully elicits looks at the foil item in all groups), although processing appears to be slower, and anticipatory strategies differ slightly from those of adults (as the color of the first-mentioned adjective in a trial elicits looks at the color-matched distractor in adults, but not in children). An analysis contrasting comprehension data for both dialects, as well as a preliminary analysis of production data will be presented at the conference.
  •  
8.
  • Ambrazaitis, Gilbert, 1979-, et al. (författare)
  • Språkutveckling och prosodisk typologi : Produktion och perception av den dialektspecifika fokusmarkeringen hos 3- till 5-åriga barn från Stockholm och Skåne
  • 2023
  • Konferensbidrag (populärvet., debatt m.m.)abstract
    • Att lägga satsbetoningen på ordet som bör fokuseras utifrån en given informationsstruktur är något som barn enligt tidigare internationell forskning verkar bemästra relativt sent i utvecklingen av sitt förstaspråk. Förmågan att uppfatta och tolka satsbetoning verkar barn enligt vissa studier utveckla ännu senare, men få tidigare studier har genomfört parallella produktions- och perceptionstest, och få perceptionsstudier har i detta sammanhang använt sig av online metoder (så som ögonrörelsemätning) med barn yngre än sex år. En del tidigare studier har påvisat typologiska skillnader; till exempel verkar svenska barn kunna producera en vuxenlik fokusbetoning tidigare än nederländska barn. Med vår studie ämnar vi bidra till en bättre förståelse av hur barn utvecklar sin förmåga att producera och uppfatta kontrastiva fokusbetoningar (t. ex. en röd BIL, en RÖD bil), hur produktionen och perceptionen hänger ihop, och vilken roll prosodisk språktypologi kan spela i sammanhanget. Vi undersöker dessa förmågor hos (totalt ca 120) förskolebarn med stockholmsvenska respektive skånska som förstaspråk – två dialekter som representerar motpoler i en prosodisk typologi när det gäller hur satsbetoning kodas intonationsfonologiskt. I vår produktionsstudie eliciterar vi adjektiv-substantiv fraser med varierad kontrastiv fokus, och i perceptionsstudien mäter vi barnens bearbetning av prosodin i liknande fraser med hjälp av ögonrörelsemätning. Datainsamling pågår i skrivande stund, och prelimära resultat kommer att presenteras på workshopen
  •  
9.
  • Andrén, Mats, et al. (författare)
  • Att lära sig språk
  • 2013
  • Ingår i: Språket, människan och världen : människans språk 1-2 - människans språk 1-2. - Lund : Studentlitteratur AB. - 9789144083391 ; , s. 73-89
  • Bokkapitel (populärvet., debatt m.m.)
  •  
10.
  • Aregger Lundh, Stefan, et al. (författare)
  • Life satisfaction in cardiac arrest survivors : A nationwide Swedish registry study
  • 2023
  • Ingår i: Resuscitation Plus. - : Elsevier. - 2666-5204. ; 15
  • Tidskriftsartikel (refereegranskat)abstract
    • IntroductionMost cardiac arrest (CA) survivors report good health and quality of life. Life satisfaction on the other hand has not yet been studied in a large scale in the CA population. We aimed to explore life satisfaction as perceived by CA survivors with three research questions addressed: (1) how do CA survivors report their life satisfaction, (2) how are different domains of life satisfaction associated with overall life satisfaction, and (3) how are demographic and medical factors associated with overall life satisfaction?MethodsThis registry study had a cross-sectional design. Life satisfaction was assessed using the 11-item Life Satisfaction checklist (LiSat-11). The sample included 1435 survivors ≥18 years of age. Descriptive statistics and binary logistic regression analyses were used.ResultsSurvivors were most satisfied with partner relation (85.6%), family life (82.2%), and self-care (77.8%), while 60.5% were satisfied with overall life. Satisfaction with psychological health was strongest associated with overall life satisfaction. Among medical and demographic factors, female sex and poor cerebral performance were associated with less overall life satisfaction.ConclusionsGenerally, CA survivors seem to perceive similar levels of overall life satisfaction as general populations, while survivors tend to be significantly less satisfied with their sexual life. Satisfaction with psychological health is of special interest to identify and treat. Additionally, female survivors and survivors with poor neurological outcome are at risk for poorer overall life satisfaction and need special attention by healthcare professionals.
  •  
Skapa referenser, mejla, bekava och länka
  • Resultat 1-10 av 217
Typ av publikation
tidskriftsartikel (125)
konferensbidrag (54)
bokkapitel (15)
rapport (8)
forskningsöversikt (6)
doktorsavhandling (5)
visa fler...
bok (2)
licentiatavhandling (2)
visa färre...
Typ av innehåll
refereegranskat (168)
övrigt vetenskapligt/konstnärligt (41)
populärvet., debatt m.m. (8)
Författare/redaktör
Ekstrand, Britten (31)
Umans, Timurs, 1981- (21)
Schmidt, Manuela (14)
Gunnarsson, Gunnar (13)
Olsen, Björn (12)
Waldenström, Jonas (10)
visa fler...
Hagell, Peter (9)
Smith, Elin (9)
Latorre-Margalef, Ne ... (9)
Broberg, Pernilla, 1 ... (8)
Elmberg, Johan (8)
Collin, Sven-Olof Yr ... (8)
Årestedt, Kristofer, ... (7)
Redfors, Andreas (7)
Westergren, Albert (6)
Linder, Cedric (6)
Umans, Timurs (6)
Behm, Lina (6)
Eriksson, Urban (6)
Bengtsson Tops, Anit ... (6)
Blomqvist, Kerstin (6)
Waldenström, Jonas, ... (5)
Airey, John (5)
Basic, Goran, 1972- (5)
Yakhlef, Sophia (5)
Hellström, Amanda (4)
Grossi, Giuseppe (4)
Sayehli, Susan (4)
Collin, Sven-Olof Yr ... (4)
Ahlström, Gerd (4)
Pajalic, Zada (4)
Andersson, Håkan S., ... (4)
Ambrazaitis, Gilbert ... (4)
Bertilsson, Charlott ... (4)
Löhndorf, Simone (4)
Tolf, Conny (4)
Furenbäck, Ingela, 1 ... (4)
Tagesson, Torbjörn (4)
Djerf, Henric (4)
Blücher, Anna (4)
Årestedt, Kristofer (3)
Uman, Timur, 1981- (3)
Saveman, Britt-inger (3)
Wallerstedt, Birgitt ... (3)
Bengtsson, Daniel (3)
H. Romøren, Anna-Sar ... (3)
Althaus, Nadja (3)
Johansson, Kjell (3)
Mohlin, Camilla, 197 ... (3)
Svensson, Britt-Mari ... (3)
visa färre...
Lärosäte
Högskolan Kristianstad (217)
Jönköping University (44)
Lunds universitet (35)
Uppsala universitet (25)
Linköpings universitet (18)
visa fler...
Högskolan i Halmstad (9)
Malmö universitet (9)
Umeå universitet (6)
Örebro universitet (6)
Karolinska Institutet (6)
Sveriges Lantbruksuniversitet (6)
Marie Cederschiöld högskola (5)
Göteborgs universitet (2)
Mälardalens universitet (2)
RISE (2)
Stockholms universitet (1)
Högskolan i Gävle (1)
Högskolan i Borås (1)
Naturhistoriska riksmuseet (1)
Blekinge Tekniska Högskola (1)
visa färre...
Språk
Engelska (177)
Svenska (40)
Forskningsämne (UKÄ/SCB)
Samhällsvetenskap (140)
Medicin och hälsovetenskap (65)
Naturvetenskap (31)
Humaniora (11)
Lantbruksvetenskap (5)
Teknik (4)

År

Kungliga biblioteket hanterar dina personuppgifter i enlighet med EU:s dataskyddsförordning (2018), GDPR. Läs mer om hur det funkar här.
Så här hanterar KB dina uppgifter vid användning av denna tjänst.

 
pil uppåt Stäng

Kopiera och spara länken för att återkomma till aktuell vy