SwePub
Sök i SwePub databas

  Extended search

Träfflista för sökning "WFRF:(Västfjäll Daniel 1975 ) srt2:(2015-2019)"

Search: WFRF:(Västfjäll Daniel 1975 ) > (2015-2019)

  • Result 1-10 of 18
Sort/group result
   
EnumerationReferenceCoverFind
1.
  • Asutay, Erkin, 1982, et al. (author)
  • Attentional and emotional prioritization of the sounds occurring outside the visual field
  • 2015
  • In: Emotion. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 1528-3542 .- 1931-1516. ; 15:3, s. 281-286
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • The ability to detect and localize sounds in an environment is critical for survival. Localizing sound sources is a computational challenge for the human brain because the auditory cortex seems to lack a topographical space representation. However, attention and task demands can modulate localization performance. Here, we investigated whether the localization performance for sounds occurring directly in front of or behind people could be modulated by emotional salience and sound-source location. We measured auditory-induced emotion by ecological sounds occurring in the frontal or rear perceptual fields, and employed a speeded localization task. The results showed that both localization speed and accuracy were higher, and that stronger negative emotions were induced when sound sources were behind the participants. Our results provide clear behavioral evidence that auditory attention can be influenced by sound-source location. Importantly, we also show that the effect of spatial location on attention is mediated by emotion, which is in line with the argument that emotional information is prioritized in processing. Auditory system functions as an alarm system and is in charge of detecting possible salient events, and alarming for an attention shift. Further, spatial processing in the auditory dorsal pathway has a function of guiding the visual system to a particular location of interest. Thus, an auditory bias toward the space outside the visual field can be useful, so that visual attention could be quickly shifted in case of emotionally significant information.
  •  
2.
  • Asutay, Erkin, 1982, et al. (author)
  • Auditory attentional selection is biased by reward cues
  • 2016
  • In: Scientific Reports. - : Springer Science and Business Media LLC. - 2045-2322 .- 2045-2322. ; 6
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Auditory attention theories suggest that humans are able to decompose the complex acoustic input into separate auditory streams, which then compete for attentional resources. How this attentional competition is influenced by motivational salience of sounds is, however, not well-understood. Here, we investigated whether a positive motivational value associated with sounds could bias the attentional selection in an auditory detection task. Participants went through a reward-learning period, where correct attentional selection of one stimulus (CS+) lead to higher rewards compared to another stimulus (CS-). We assessed the impact of reward-learning by comparing perceptual sensitivity before and after the learning period, when CS+ and CS-were presented as distractors for a different target. Performance decreased after reward-learning when CS+ was a distractor, while it increased when CS- was a distractor. Thus, the findings show that sounds that were associated with high rewards captures attention involuntarily. Additionally, when successful inhibition of a particular sound (CS-) was associated with high rewards then it became easier to ignore it. The current findings have important implications for the understanding of the organizing principles of auditory perception and provide, for the first time, clear behavioral evidence for reward-dependent attentional learning in the auditory domain in humans.
  •  
3.
  • Asutay, Erkin, 1982, et al. (author)
  • Negative emotion provides cues for orienting auditory spatial attention
  • 2015
  • In: Frontiers in Psychology. - : Frontiers Media SA. - 1664-1078. ; 6:MAY
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • The auditory stimuli provide information about the objects and events around us. They can also carry biologically significant emotional information (such as unseen dangers and conspecific vocalizations), which provides cues for allocation of attention and mental resources. Here, we investigated whether task-irrelevant auditory emotional information can provide cues for orientation of auditory spatial attention. We employed a covert spatial orienting task: the dot-probe task. In each trial, two task irrelevant auditory cues were simultaneously presented at two separate locations (left-right or front-back). Environmental sounds were selected to form emotional vs. neutral, emotional vs. emotional, and neutral vs. neutral cue pairs. The participants’ task was to detect the location of an acoustic target that was presented immediately after the task-irrelevant auditory cues. The target was presented at the same location as one of the auditory cues. The results indicated that participants were significantly faster to locate the target when it replaced the negative cue compared to when it replaced the neutral cue. The positive cues did not produce a clear attentional bias. Further, same valence pairs (emotional-emotional or neutral-neutral) did not modulate reaction times due to a lack of spatial attention capture by one cue in the pair. Taken together, the results indicate that negative affect can provide cues for the orientation of spatial attention in the auditory domain.
  •  
4.
  • Asutay, Erkin, 1982-, et al. (author)
  • Sound and emotion
  • 2019
  • In: The Oxford Handbook of Sound and Imagination Vol.2. - New York : Oxford University Press. - 9780190460273 - 9780190460242 ; , s. 368-390
  • Book chapter (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • The focus of Erkin Asutay and Daniel Västfjäll’s chapter is the relationship between sound and emotion. Evidence from behavioral and neuroimaging studies is presented that documents how sound can evoke emotions and how emotional processes affect sound perception. This leads to a discussion of different forms of emotional responses to auditory stimuli, such as responses to vocal signals, responses to environmental sounds, and responses to music. The authors view the auditory system as an adaptive network that governs both how auditory stimuli influence emotional reactions and how the affective significance of sound influences auditory attention. In conclusion, they argue that affective experience is integral to auditory perception.
  •  
5.
  • Bjälkebring, Pär, 1984, et al. (author)
  • Greater Emotional Gain from Giving in Older Adults: Age-Related Positivity Bias in Charitable Giving
  • 2016
  • In: Frontiers in Psychology. - : Frontiers Media SA. - 1664-1078. ; 7
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Older adults have been shown to avoid negative and prefer positive information to a higher extent than younger adults. This positivity bias influences their information processing as well as decision-making. We investigate age-related positivity bias in charitable giving in two studies. In Study 1 we examine motivational factors in monetary donations, while Study 2 focuses on the emotional effect of actual monetary donations. In Study 1, participants (n = 353, age range 20-74 years) were asked to rate their affect toward a person in need and then state how much money they would be willing to donate to help this person. In Study 2, participants (n = 108, age range 19-89) were asked to rate their affect toward a donation made a few days prior. Regression analysis was used to investigate whether or not the positivity bias influences the relationship between affect and donations. In Study 1, we found that older adults felt more sympathy and compassion and were less motivated by negative affect when compared to younger adults, who were motivated by both negative and positive affect. In Study 2, we found that the level of positive emotional reactions from monetary donations was higher in older participants compared to younger participants. We find support for an age-related positivity bias in charitable giving. This is true for motivation to make a future donation, as well as affective thinking about a previous donation. We conclude that older adults draw more positive affect from both the planning and outcome of monetary donations and hence benefit more from engaging in monetary charity than their younger counterparts.
  •  
6.
  • Bjälkebring, Pär, 1984, et al. (author)
  • Regulation of Experienced and Anticipated Regret in Daily Decision Making
  • 2016
  • In: Emotion. - : American Psychological Association (APA). - 1528-3542 .- 1931-1516. ; 16:3, s. 381-386
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Decisions were sampled from 108 participants during 8 days using a web-based diary method. Each day participants rated experienced regret for a decision made, as well as forecasted regret for a decision to be made. Participants also indicated to what extent they used different strategies to prevent or regulate regret. Participants regretted 30% of decisions and forecasted regret in 70% of future decisions, indicating both that regret is relatively prevalent in daily decisions but also that experienced regret was less frequent than forecasted regret. In addition, a number of decision-specific regulation and prevention strategies were successfully used by the participants to minimize regret and negative emotions in daily decision making. Overall, these results suggest that regulation and prevention of regret are important strategies in many of our daily decisions. Regulation of Experienced and Anticipated Regret in Daily Decision Making. Available from: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/283425347_Regulation_of_Experienced_and_Anticipated_Regret_in_Daily_Decision_Making [accessed Dec 3, 2015].
  •  
7.
  • Erlandsson, Arvid, 1982-, et al. (author)
  • Attitudes and donation behavior when reading positive and negative charity appeals
  • 2018
  • In: Journal of Nonprofit & Public Sector Marketing. - : Routledge. - 1049-5142 .- 1540-6997. ; 30:4, s. 444-474
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • This article tries to clarify whether negative charity appeals (i.e., advertisements emphasizing the bad consequences of not helping) or positive charity appeals (i.e., advertisements emphasizing the good consequences of helping) are more effective. Previous literature does not provide a single answer to this question and we suggest that one contributing reason for this is that different studies have operationalized appeal effectiveness in different ways (e.g., actual behavior, self-rated helping intentions, or expressed attitudes about the ad or the organization). Results from four separate studies suggest that positive appeals are more effective in inducing favorable attitudes toward the ad and toward the organization but that negative appeals are more effective (in studies 1A and 1B) or at least equally effective (in studies 1C and 1D) in eliciting actual donations. Also, although people’s attitude toward the appeal (i.e., liking) was a good predictor for the expected effectiveness in increasing donation behavior (in Study 2), it was a poor predictor of actual donation behavior in all four main studies. These results cast doubt on marketing theories suggesting that attitudes toward an advertisement and toward the brand always lead to higher purchase behavior.
  •  
8.
  • Erlandsson, Arvid, 1982-, et al. (author)
  • Bullshit-sensitivity predicts prosocial behavior
  • 2018
  • In: PLOS ONE. - San Francisco, United States : Public Library of Science. - 1932-6203. ; 13:7
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Bullshit-sensitivity is the ability to distinguish pseudo-profound bullshit sentences (e.g. “Your movement transforms universal observations”) from genuinely profound sentences (e.g. “The person who never made a mistake never tried something new”). Although bullshit-sensitivity has been linked to other individual difference measures, it has not yet been shown to predict any actual behavior. We therefore conducted a survey study with over a thousand participants from a general sample of the Swedish population and assessed participants’ bullshit-receptivity (i.e. their perceived meaningfulness of seven bullshit sentences) and profoundness-receptivity (i.e. their perceived meaningfulness of seven genuinely profound sentences), and used these variables to predict two types of prosocial behavior (self-reported donations and a decision to volunteer for charity). Despite bullshit-receptivity and profoundness-receptivity being positively correlated with each other, logistic regression analyses showed that profoundness-receptivity had a positive association whereas bullshit-receptivity had a negative association with both types of prosocial behavior. These relations held up for the most part when controlling for potentially intermediating factors such as cognitive ability, time spent completing the survey, sex, age, level of education, and religiosity. The results suggest that people who are better at distinguishing the pseudo-profound from the actually profound are more prosocial.
  •  
9.
  • Hagman, William, 1987-, et al. (author)
  • Public Views on Policies Involving Nudges
  • 2015
  • In: Review of Philosophy and Psychology. - : Springer Science and Business Media LLC. - 1878-5158 .- 1878-5166. ; 6:3, s. 439-453
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • When should nudging be deemed as permissible and when should it be deemed as intrusive to individuals’ freedom of choice? Should all types of nudges be judged the same? To date the debate concerning these issues has largely proceeded without much input from the general public. The main objective of this study is to elicit public views on the use of nudges in policy. In particular we investigate attitudes toward two broad categories of nudges that we label pro-self (i.e. focusing on private welfare) and pro-social (i.e. focusing on social welfare) nudges. In addition we explore how individual differences in thinking and feeling influence attitudes toward nudges. General population samples in Sweden and the United States (n=952) were presented with vignettes describing nudge-policies and rated acceptability and intrusiveness on freedom of choice. To test for individual differences, measures on cultural cognition and analytical thinking were included. Results show that the level of acceptance toward nudge-policies was generally high in both countries, but were slightly higher among Swedes than Americans. Somewhat paradoxically a majority of the respondents also perceived the presented nudge-policies as intrusive to freedom of choice. Nudge- polices classified as pro-social had a significantly lower acceptance rate compared to pro-self nudges (p<.0001). Individuals with a more individualistic worldview were less likely to perceive nudges as acceptable, while individuals more prone to analytical thinking were less likely to perceive nudges as intrusive to freedom of choice. To conclude, our findings suggest that the notion of “one-nudge- fits-all” is not tenable. Recognizing this is an important aspect both for successfully implementing nudges as well as nuancing nudge theory. 
  •  
10.
  • Hagman, William, 1987- (author)
  • When are nudges acceptable? : Influences of beneficiaries, techniques, alternatives and choice architects
  • 2018
  • Doctoral thesis (other academic/artistic)abstract
    • Interventions aimed to change behavior (so called nudges) are becoming more and more popular among policymakers. However, in order to be able to effectively use nudges, it is important to understand when and why people find them acceptable. The objective of this thesis is therefore to improve the understanding of when nudges are judged to be acceptable. The thesis focuses on a model for behavioral change. The model contains two parts, nudge technique and acceptance of nudges. Nudge technique refers to how the nudge is designed to function in regard to psychological mechanism and functionality.The nudge technique part of the model is expanded and problematized from an ethical perspective in the first part of this thesis, by exemplifying psychological mechanisms behind different techniques and explaining why they might be intrusive to individuals’ freedom of choice. In the second part of this thesis it is discussed why acceptance is an important component of making nudging legitimate and effective. This is followed by a discussion of how acceptance is empirically measured. The empirical part of the thesis is based on four papers which all use a quantitative online survey approach to study the judgements of nudges from the general public.Paper 1 was a first attempt to measure whether nudges which are common in the nudge literature are acceptable interventions according to the general public. We found that the nudges that were categorized as pro-self were more likely to be rated as acceptable and less likely to be perceived as intrusive to freedom of choice compared to pro-social nudges. Furthermore, the effect of decision styles and worldview on acceptance was explored. In paper 2, we explored whether the difference between acceptance found for pro-social nudges and proself nudges could be increased by framing nudges as beneficial for society or individuals. The framing had no effect on acceptance but, as in paper 1, pro-social nudges were found to be more intrusive to freedom of choice compared to pro-self framed nudges. Moreover, different nudge techniques had different rates of acceptance even with the same explicit goal for the nudges. In paper 3, we examined whether the alternative to nudges affects the perceived acceptability and intrusiveness of default-changing nudge techniques. The alternatives given to the nudges were either to enforce the intended behavioral change with legislation or to do nothing at all in order to change the behavior. We find no difference in aggregated acceptance, however, the judgements vary depending on individuals’ worldview. Paper 4 explored if the choice architect’s (the creator/proposer of the nudge) political affiliation affects acceptance rating for proposed nudge interventions and legislation. We find that acceptance of both nudges and legislation increases with the level of matching between people’s political orientation and the choice architect’s political affiliation.Taken together, the findings suggest that there is more to creating an acceptable nudge than to merely take a nudge technique that was acceptable in one context and apply it in another. Moreover, nudges that are rated as more beneficial towards individuals compared to society at large are in general more likely to be found acceptable and less intrusive to freedom of choice. It is important to have knowledge about the target population (e.g. their decision styles, world-views, and political orientation) to avoid backfires when implementing nudges.  
  •  
Skapa referenser, mejla, bekava och länka
  • Result 1-10 of 18
Type of publication
journal article (14)
doctoral thesis (2)
reports (1)
book chapter (1)
Type of content
peer-reviewed (15)
other academic/artistic (3)
Author/Editor
Västfjäll, Daniel, 1 ... (17)
Tinghög, Gustav, 197 ... (7)
Asutay, Erkin, 1982 (4)
Aczel, Balazs (3)
Bjälkebring, Pär, 19 ... (3)
Baskin, Ernest (3)
show more...
Koppel, Lina, 1988- (3)
Birt, Angie R. (3)
Coary, Sean P. (3)
Holzmeister, Felix (2)
Huber, Juergen (2)
Kirchler, Michael (2)
Vanpaemel, Wolf (2)
Voracek, Martin (2)
Erlandsson, Arvid, 1 ... (2)
Slovic, Paul (2)
Roets, Arne (2)
Mechtel, Mario (2)
Scopelliti, Irene (2)
Warmelink, Lara (2)
Szecsi, Peter (2)
Kovacs, Marton (2)
Wiggins, Bradford J. (2)
Bakos, Bence E. (2)
Evans, Jacqueline R. (2)
Hagman, William, 198 ... (2)
Barbosa, Fernando (2)
McCarthy, Randy J (2)
Verschuere, Bruno (2)
Skowronski, John J. (2)
Meijer, Ewout H. (2)
Jim, Ariane (2)
Hoogesteyn, Katherin ... (2)
Orthey, Robin (2)
Acar, Oguz A. (2)
Bègue, Laurent (2)
Ben-Shakhar, Gershon (2)
Blatz, Lisa (2)
Charman, Steve D. (2)
Claesen, Aline (2)
Clay, Samuel L. (2)
Crusius, Jan (2)
Feldman, Noa (2)
Ferreira-Santos, Fer ... (2)
Gamer, Matthias (2)
Gomes, Sara (2)
González-Iraizoz, Ma ... (2)
Isoni, Andrea (2)
Jessup, Ryan K. (2)
Laine, Tei (2)
show less...
University
Linköping University (17)
Chalmers University of Technology (6)
University of Gothenburg (3)
Uppsala University (1)
Stockholm University (1)
Lund University (1)
Language
English (18)
Research subject (UKÄ/SCB)
Social Sciences (18)
Medical and Health Sciences (4)
Natural sciences (1)

Year

Kungliga biblioteket hanterar dina personuppgifter i enlighet med EU:s dataskyddsförordning (2018), GDPR. Läs mer om hur det funkar här.
Så här hanterar KB dina uppgifter vid användning av denna tjänst.

 
pil uppåt Close

Copy and save the link in order to return to this view