SwePub
Sök i SwePub databas

  Extended search

Träfflista för sökning "WFRF:(Isager Peder M.) "

Search: WFRF:(Isager Peder M.)

  • Result 1-4 of 4
Sort/group result
   
EnumerationReferenceCoverFind
1.
  • Botvinik-Nezer, Rotem, et al. (author)
  • Variability in the analysis of a single neuroimaging dataset by many teams
  • 2020
  • In: Nature. - : Springer Science and Business Media LLC. - 0028-0836 .- 1476-4687. ; 582, s. 84-88
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Data analysis workflows in many scientific domains have become increasingly complex and flexible. Here we assess the effect of this flexibility on the results of functional magnetic resonance imaging by asking 70 independent teams to analyse the same dataset, testing the same 9 ex-ante hypotheses(1). The flexibility of analytical approaches is exemplified by the fact that no two teams chose identical workflows to analyse the data. This flexibility resulted in sizeable variation in the results of hypothesis tests, even for teams whose statistical maps were highly correlated at intermediate stages of the analysis pipeline. Variation in reported results was related to several aspects of analysis methodology. Notably, a meta-analytical approach that aggregated information across teams yielded a significant consensus in activated regions. Furthermore, prediction markets of researchers in the field revealed an overestimation of the likelihood of significant findings, even by researchers with direct knowledge of the dataset(2-5). Our findings show that analytical flexibility can have substantial effects on scientific conclusions, and identify factors that may be related to variability in the analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging. The results emphasize the importance of validating and sharing complex analysis workflows, and demonstrate the need for performing and reporting multiple analyses of the same data. Potential approaches that could be used to mitigate issues related to analytical variability are discussed. The results obtained by seventy different teams analysing the same functional magnetic resonance imaging dataset show substantial variation, highlighting the influence of analytical choices and the importance of sharing workflows publicly and performing multiple analyses.
  •  
2.
  • Moshontz, Hannah, et al. (author)
  • The Psychological Science Accelerator: Advancing Psychology Through a Distributed Collaborative Network
  • 2018
  • In: Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science. - : SAGE Publications. - 2515-2459 .- 2515-2467. ; 1:4, s. 501-515
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • Concerns about the veracity of psychological research have been growing. Many findings in psychological science are based on studies with insufficient statistical power and nonrepresentative samples, or may otherwise be limited to specific, ungeneralizable settings or populations. Crowdsourced research, a type of large-scale collaboration in which one or more research projects are conducted across multiple lab sites, offers a pragmatic solution to these and other current methodological challenges. The Psychological Science Accelerator (PSA) is a distributed network of laboratories designed to enable and support crowdsourced research projects. These projects can focus on novel research questions or replicate prior research in large, diverse samples. The PSA’s mission is to accelerate the accumulation of reliable and generalizable evidence in psychological science. Here, we describe the background, structure, principles, procedures, benefits, and challenges of the PSA. In contrast to other crowdsourced research networks, the PSA is ongoing (as opposed to time limited), efficient (in that structures and principles are reused for different projects), decentralized, diverse (in both subjects and researchers), and inclusive (of proposals, contributions, and other relevant input from anyone inside or outside the network). The PSA and other approaches to crowdsourced psychological science will advance understanding of mental processes and behaviors by enabling rigorous research and systematic examination of its generalizability.
  •  
3.
  • Lakens, Daniel, et al. (author)
  • Justify your alpha
  • 2018
  • In: Nature Human Behaviour. - : Nature Publishing Group. - 2397-3374. ; 2:3, s. 168-171
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • In response to recommendations to redefine statistical significance to P ≤ 0.005, we propose that researchers should transparently report and justify all choices they make when designing a study, including the alpha level.
  •  
4.
  • Buchanan, Erin M., et al. (author)
  • Getting Started Creating Data Dictionaries : How to Create a Shareable Data Set
  • 2021
  • In: Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science. - : Sage Publications. - 2515-2459 .- 2515-2467. ; 4:1
  • Journal article (peer-reviewed)abstract
    • As researchers embrace open and transparent data sharing, they will need to provide information about their data that effectively helps others understand their data sets’ contents. Without proper documentation, data stored in online repositories such as OSF will often be rendered unfindable and unreadable by other researchers and indexing search engines. Data dictionaries and codebooks provide a wealth of information about variables, data collection, and other important facets of a data set. This information, called metadata, provides key insights into how the data might be further used in research and facilitates search-engine indexing to reach a broader audience of interested parties. This Tutorial first explains terminology and standards relevant to data dictionaries and codebooks. Accompanying information on OSF presents a guided workflow of the entire process from source data (e.g., survey answers on Qualtrics) to an openly shared data set accompanied by a data dictionary or codebook that follows an agreed-upon standard. Finally, we discuss freely available Web applications to assist this process of ensuring that psychology data are findable, accessible, interoperable, and reusable.
  •  
Skapa referenser, mejla, bekava och länka
  • Result 1-4 of 4
Type of publication
journal article (4)
Type of content
peer-reviewed (4)
Author/Editor
Aczel, Balazs (2)
Carlsson, Rickard, 1 ... (2)
Kenny, David A. (1)
Johansson, Christer (1)
Nilsonne, Gustav (1)
Albers, Casper J. (1)
show more...
Botvinik-Nezer, Rote ... (1)
Dreber Almenberg, An ... (1)
Holzmeister, Felix (1)
Huber, Juergen (1)
Johannesson, Magnus (1)
Kirchler, Michael (1)
Poldrack, Russell A. (1)
Schonberg, Tom (1)
Sullivan, Gavin Bren ... (1)
Tinghög, Gustav, 197 ... (1)
Glerean, Enrico (1)
Ingre, Michael (1)
Danielsson, Henrik (1)
Zhang, Lei (1)
Chartier, Christophe ... (1)
Grange, James A. (1)
Hasselman, Fred (1)
Levitan, Carmel A. (1)
Miller, Jeremy K. (1)
Schmidt, Kathleen (1)
Spies, Jeffrey R. (1)
Stieger, Stefan (1)
van Assen, Marcel A. ... (1)
Vanpaemel, Wolf (1)
Vianello, Michelange ... (1)
Voracek, Martin (1)
Ansari, Daniel (1)
Fiedler, Susann (1)
Heunis, Stephan (1)
Sjoerds, Zsuzsika (1)
Cunningham, William ... (1)
Lamm, Claus (1)
Zwaan, Rolf A. (1)
Hamilton, Paul J., 1 ... (1)
Durnez, Joke (1)
Olsen, Jerome (1)
Schei, Vidar (1)
Zhang, Xu (1)
Kekecs, Zoltan (1)
Camerer, Colin F. (1)
Iwanir, Roni (1)
Mumford, Jeanette A. (1)
Adcock, R. Alison (1)
Avesani, Paolo (1)
show less...
University
Stockholm University (2)
Linköping University (2)
Linnaeus University (2)
Karolinska Institutet (2)
Lund University (1)
Stockholm School of Economics (1)
Language
English (4)
Research subject (UKÄ/SCB)
Social Sciences (4)
Natural sciences (2)
Medical and Health Sciences (2)

Year

Kungliga biblioteket hanterar dina personuppgifter i enlighet med EU:s dataskyddsförordning (2018), GDPR. Läs mer om hur det funkar här.
Så här hanterar KB dina uppgifter vid användning av denna tjänst.

 
pil uppåt Close

Copy and save the link in order to return to this view